References

References

Ambridge, B. & Lieven, E. V.
(2011) Child language acquisition: Contrasting theoretical approaches. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderssen, M., Bentzen, K., & Rodina, Y.
(2012) Topicality and complexity in the acquisition of norwegian object shift. Language Acquisition, 19(1), 39–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, S. L., Gleitman, L. R., & Gleitman, H.
(1983) What some concepts might not be. Cognition, 13(3), 263–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., Neely, J. H., Nelson, D. L., Simpson, G. B., & Treiman, R.
(2007) The English Lexicon Project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berko, J.
(1958) The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14(2–3), 150–177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berwick, R. C. & Chomsky, N.
(2016) Why only us: Language and evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Biberauer, T.
(2018) Less is More: On the Tolerance Principle as a manifestation of Maximize Minimal Means. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 707–711. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boyd, J. K. & Goldberg, A. E.
(2011) Learning what not to say: The role of statistical preemption and categorization in a-adjective production. Language, 87(1), 55–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brannon, E. M., Wusthoff, C. J., Gallistel, C., & Gibbon, J.
(2001) Numerical subtraction in the pigeon: Evidence for a linear subjective number scale. Psychological Science, 12(3), 238–243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ceolin, A.
(2018) Explaining cross-linguistic differences in article omission through an acquisition model. In Bertolini, A. B. & Kaplan, M. G., (Eds.), Proceedings of the 42nd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, pp. 100–113, Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1968) Language and mind. Harcourt, Brace and World.Google Scholar
(1995) The minimalist program. Boston: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Derks, P. L. & Paclisanu, M. I.
(1967) Simple strategies in binary prediction by children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(2), 278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Cat, C.
(2018) Evaluating Yang’s algorithms: An outline. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 712–716. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dimroth, C.
(2018) Input and the acquisition of productive grammatical knowledge: Vocabulary size as missing link? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 722–726. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Domínguez, L. & González Alonso, J.
(2018) What is the role of L1 representations in a grammar-input model of L2 acquisition? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 717–721. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fruchter, J. & Marantz, A.
(2015) Decomposition, lookup, and recombination: MEG evidence for the Full Decomposition model of complex visual word recognition. Brain and Language, 1431, 81–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. & Gelman, R.
(1992) Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition, 44(1–2), 43–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbon, J.
(1977) Scalar expectancy theory and weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84(3), 279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L.
(1990) The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition, 1(1), 3–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L. R., Cassidy, K., Nappa, R., Papafragou, A., & Trueswell, J. C.
(2005) Hard words. Language Learning and Development, 1(1), 23–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A.
(2018) The sufficiency principle hyperinflates the price of productivity. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 727–732.Google Scholar
Gries, S. Th.
(2018) Mechanistic formal approaches to language acquisition: Yes, but at the right level(s) of resolution. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 733–737.Google Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, T. R.
(1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H Brookes Publishing, Baltimore, MD.Google Scholar
Hudson Kam, C. L. & Newport, E. L.
(2005) Regularizing unpredictable variation: The roles of adult and child learners in language formation and change. Language Learning and Development, 1(2), 151–195. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jarmulowicz, L.
(2002) English derivational suffix frequency and children’s stress judgements. Brain and Language, 81(1–3), 192–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jäschke, T. & Plag, I.
(2016) The dative alternation in German-English interlanguage. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 381, 485–521. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kapatsinski, V.
(2018) On the intolerance of the Tolerance Principle. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 738–742.Google Scholar
Lidz, J. & Perkins, L.
(2018) The importance of input representations. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 743–748.Google Scholar
Lignos, C.
(2013) Modeling words in the mind. PhD thesis, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Fernald, A., & Hurtado, N.
(2010) How vocabulary size in two languages relates to efficiency in spoken word recognition by young spanish-english bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 37(4), 817–840. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S.
(2018) Learning a Second Language Takes More than Math. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 749–752. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J.
(2018) Language-level input factors are not enough to explain child bilingual acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 753–757. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pine, J. M., Freudenthal, D., Krajewski, G., & Gobet, F.
(2013) Do young children have adultlike syntactic categories? Zipf’s law and the case of the determiner. Cognition, 127(3), 345–360. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Regel, S., Opitz, A., Müller, G., & Friederici, A. D.
(2015) The past tense debate revisited: Electrophysiological evidence for subregularities of irregular verb inflection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(9), 1870–1885. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roeper, T.
(2018) Grammar acquisition and grammar choice in the variationist model. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 758–763.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. & Chomsky, N.
(2018) Towards eliminating arbitrary stipulations related to parameters: Linguistic innateness and the variational model. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 764–769.Google Scholar
Rowland, C.
(2018) The principles of scientific inquiry. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 770–775.Google Scholar
Schuler, K.
(2017) The acquisition of productive rules in child and adult language learners. PhD thesis, Georgetown University, Washington, D.C.Google Scholar
Schütze, C. T.
(2005) Thinking about what we are asking speakers to do. In Kepser, S. & Reis, M., (eds.), Linguistic evidence: Empirical, theoretical, and computational perspectives, pages 457–485. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slabakova, R.
(2018) Back to our roots. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 776–780. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, S.
(1969) Memory-scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 57(4), 421–457.Google Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, M.
(1998) The particulate origins of language generativity: from syllable to gesture. In Hurford, J., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Knight, C., (eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language, pages 202–221. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Svenonius, P.
(2018) Learning rules versus learning items. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 781–786. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taft, M.
(2004) Morphological decomposition and the reverse base frequency effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A(4), 745–765. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Lin, Y., Armstrong, B., Cartmill, E. A., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Gleitman, L. R.
(2016) Perceiving referential intent: Dynamics of reference in natural parent-child interactions. Cognition, 1481, 117–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tyler, A. & Nagy, W.
(1989) The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28(6):649–667. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Visser, F. T.
(1963) An historical syntax of the English language. Brill Archive.Google Scholar
Weir, M. W.
(1964) Developmental changes in problem-solving strategies. Psychological Review, 71(6), 473. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L. & Genesee, F.
(1996) How native is near-native? the issue of ultimate attainment in adult second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 12(3), 233–265. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wittenberg, E. & Jackendoff, R.
(2018) Formalist modeling and psychological reality. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 787–791. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Xu, F. & Pinker, S.
(1995) Weird past tense forms. Journal of Child Language, 22(3), 531–556. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, C.
(2002) Knowledge and learning in natural language. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
(2013a) Ontogeny and phylogeny of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(16), 6324–6327 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013b) Who’s afraid of George Kingsley Zipf? Or: Do children and chimps have language? Significance, 10(6), 29–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Negative knowledge from positive evidence. Language, 91(4):938–953. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) The price of linguistic productivity: How children learn to break rules of language. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Rage against the machine: Evaluation metrics in the 21st century. Language Acquisition, 24(2), 100–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) A user’s guide to the Tolerance Principle. Manuscript. University of Pennsylvania ([URL]).
Yang, C., Crain, S., Berwick, R. C., Chomsky, N., & Bolhuis, J. J.
(2017) The growth of language: Universal grammar, experience, and principles of computation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 81(Part B), 103–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, C. & Montrul, S.
(2017) Learning datives: The tolerance principle in monolingual and bilingual acquisition. Second Language Research, 33(1), 119–144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, C. & Valian, V.
(2018) Determiners and grammars. Submitted.Google Scholar
Yusa, N.
(2018) Input effects on the development of I-language in L2 acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(6), 792–796. DOI logoGoogle Scholar