References (52)
References
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. M., & Walker, S. C. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C. (2006). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(1), 3–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Connine, C., Ferreira, F., Jones, C., Clifton, C., & Frazier, L. (1984). Verb frame preferences: Descriptive norms. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 13(4), 307–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2000). The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22(4), 499–533. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dekydtspotter, L., & Seo, H. K. (2017). Transitivity in the processing of intransitive clauses: A category-based prediction in low-intermediate learners of English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 39(3), 527–552. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Domínguez, L., & Arche, M. J. (2021). The ‘comparative logic’ and why we need to explain interlanguage grammars. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 717635. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., & Cramer Scaltz, T. R. (2008). Spanish – English L2 speakers’ use of subcategorization bias information in the resolution of temporary ambiguity during second language reading. Acta psychologica, 128(3), 501–513. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Marful, A., Gerfen, C., & Molina, M. T. B. (2010). Usage frequencies of complement-taking verbs in Spanish and English: Data from Spanish monolinguals and Spanish – English bilinguals. Behavior Research Methods, 42(4), 1004–1011. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fang, S., & Wu, Z. (2024). Syntactic prediction in L2 learners: evidence from English disjunction processing. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 62(2), 429-456. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frazier, L., & Fodor, J. D. (1978). The sausage machine: A new two-stage parsing model. Cognition, 6(4), 291–325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C., & Pynte, J. (1997). Syntactic ambiguity resolution while reading in second and native languages. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 50(1), 119–148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garnsey, S. M., Lotocky, M. A., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Myers, E. M. (1997a). Argument structure frequency biases for 100 sentence-complement-taking verbs |(|Unpublished manuscript). University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Garnsey, S. M., Pearlmutter, N. J., Myers, E., & Lotocky, M. A. (1997b). The contributions of verb bias and plausibility to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(1), 58–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirotani, M., Frazier, L., & Rayner, K. (2006). Punctuation and intonation effects on clause and sentence wrap-up: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(3), 425–443. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H. (2010). Ultimate attainment in L2 inflectional morphology: Performance similarities between non-native and native speakers. Lingua, 120, 901–931. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015). Individual differences in the second language processing of object – subject ambiguities. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(2), 129–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacob, G., & Felser, C. (2016). Reanalysis and semantic persistence in native and non-native garden-path recovery. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(5), 907–925. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jegerski, J. (2012). The processing of subject – object ambiguities in native and near-native Mexican Spanish. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(4), 721–735. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014). Self-paced reading. In J. Jegerski & B. VanPatten (Eds.), Research methods in second language psycholinguistics (pp. 20–49). Routledge.Google Scholar
Juffs, A. (1998). Main verb versus reduced relative clause ambiguity resolution in L2 sentence processing. Language Learning, 48(1), 107–147. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004). Representation, processing and working memory in a second language. Transactions of the Philological Society, 102(2), 199–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A., & Fang, S. (2022). A generative approach to the instructed second language acquisition of Spanish se. Language Learning, 72(S1), 83-124.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. (1996). Garden path sentences and error data in second language sentence processing. Language learning, 46(2), 283–323. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82, 1–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lau, J. H., Clark, A., & Lappin, S. (2017). Grammaticality, acceptability, and probability: A probabilistic view of linguistic knowledge. Cognitive Science, 41(5), 1202–1241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, E. K., Lu, D. H. Y., & Garnsey, S. M. (2013). L1 word order and sensitivity to verb bias in L2 processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(4), 761–775. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lemhöfer, K., & Broersma, M. (2012). Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid lexical test for advanced learners of English. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 325–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lenth, R. (2020). Emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means aka Least-Squares Means. R package (Version 1.7.0). Retrieved on 15 May 2022 from [URL]
Linck, J. A., & Cunnings, I. (2015). The utility and application of mixed‐effects models in second language research. Language Learning, 65(S1), 185–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101(4), 676. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McDonald, J. L. (2006). Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgment performance by late second language Learner. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 381–401. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicklin, C., & Plonsky, L. (2020). Outliers in L2 research in applied linguistics: A synthesis and data re-analysis. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 40, 26–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Traxler, M. J. (1998). Plausibility and recovery from garden paths: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(4), 940. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pozzan, L., & Trueswell, J. C. (2016). Second language processing and revision of garden-path sentences: a visual word study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(3), 636–643. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pritchett, B. L. (1992). Grammatical competence and parsing performance. The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Qian, Z., Lee, E. K., Lu, D. H. Y., & Garnsey, S. M. (2019). Native and non-native (L1-Mandarin) speakers of English differ in online use of verb-based cues about sentence structure. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(5), 897–911. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [URL]
Roberts, L., & Felser, C. (2011). Plausibility and recovery from garden paths in second language sentence processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(2), 299–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Román, P. E., Ray, N. R., Contemori, C., Kaan, E., & Dussias, P. E. (2013). The role of verb bias and plausibility in the resolution of temporarily ambiguous sentences: An ERP study with English speakers. Poster presented at the 54th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomics Society, Toronto, ON, Canada.Google Scholar
Román, P., Kaan, E., & Dussias, P. E. (2022). Access to verb bias and plausibility information during syntactic processing in adult Spanish – English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25(3), 417–429. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Şafak, D. F., & Hopp, H. (2022). Verb bias and semantic persistence effects in L2 ambiguity resolution. Second Language Research, 38(4), 705–736. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spinner, P., & Gass, S. M. (2019). Using judgments in second language acquisition research. Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sprouse, J. (2018). Acceptability judgments and grammaticality, prospects and challenges. Syntactic structures after 60 years (pp. 195–224). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sturt, P., Pickering, M. J., & Crocker, M. W. (1999). Structural change and reanalysis difficulty in language comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(1), 136–150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Kello, C. (1993). Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: separating effects of lexical preference from garden-paths. Journal of Experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and Cognition, 19(3), 528. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Gompel, R. P., & Pickering, M. J. (2007). Syntactic parsing. In S.-A. Rueschemeyer & M. Gareth Gaskell (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 289–307). Oxford Academic. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Gompel, R. P., Pickering, M. J., & Traxler, M. J. (2001). Reanalysis in sentence processing: Evidence against current constraint-based and two-stage models. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(2), 225–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L. D. A., François, R., … & Yutani, H. (2019). Welcome to the Tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(43), 1686. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. N., Möbius, P., & Kim, C. (2001). Native and non-native processing of English wh-questions: Parsing strategies and plausibility constraints. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22(4), 509–540. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M. P., & Garnsey, S. M. (2009). Making simple sentences hard: Verb bias effects in simple direct object sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(3), 368–392. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Witzel, J., Witzel, N., & Nicol, J. (2012). Deeper than shallow: Evidence for structure-based parsing biases in second-language sentence processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(2), 419–456. DOI logoGoogle Scholar