Part of
Multifaceted Multilingualism
Edited by Kleanthes K. Grohmann
[Studies in Bilingualism 66] 2024
► pp. 2246
References (57)
References
Abney, S. P. (1987). The English noun phrase in its sentential aspect (Unpublished PhD dissertation). Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Adger, D. (2003). Core syntax: A minimalist approach. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berwick, R. C., & Chomsky, N. (2016). Why only us: Language and evolution. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boeckx, C. (2009). The nature of Merge: Consequences for language, mind, and biology. In M. Piattelli-Palmarini, J. Uriagereka, & P. Salaburu (Eds.), Of minds and language: A dialogue with Noam Chomsky in the Basque country (pp. 44–57). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J., & Everaert, M. (2013). Birdsong, speech, and language: Exploring the evolution of mind and brain. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., & Hanlon, C. (1970). Derivational complexity and order of acquisition on child speech. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language (pp. 11–53). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2001). Beyond explanatory adequacy: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, 20.Google Scholar
(2014). Aspects of the theory of syntax. The MIT Press. (Original work published 1965).Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2011). The recursive mind: The origins of human language, thought, and civilization. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Crain, S. (1991). Language acquisition in the absence of experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14(4), 597–612. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, S., & Pietroski, P. (2001). Nature, nurture and Universal Grammar. Linguistics and Philosophy, 24(2), 139–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, S., & Thornton, R. (2000). Investigations in Universal Grammar: A guide to experiments on the acquisition of syntax and semantics. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, S., Giblin, I., & Thornton, R. (2021). The deep forces that shape language and the poverty of the stimulus. In N. Allott, T. Lohndal, & G. Rey (Eds.), A Companion to Chomsky (pp. 462–475). John Wiley & Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Culicover, P. (1976). Syntax. Academic Press.Google Scholar
den Dikken, M., & Dékány, É. (2018). A restriction on recursion. Syntax, 21, 37–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Everett, D. L. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã: Another look at the design features of human language. Current Anthropology, 46(4), 621–646. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). Language: The cultural tool. Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T., & Hauser, M. D. (2004). Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a non-human primate. Science, 303, 377–380. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. T., Hauser, M. D., & Chomsky, N. (2005). The evolution of the language faculty: Clarifications and implications. Cognition, 97, 179–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Freeze, R. (1992). Existentials and other locativesLanguage, 68, 553–595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentile, S. (2003). On the acquisition of left-branching recursive possessives. Amherst: University of Massachusetts honors thesis.Google Scholar
Giblin, I., Zhou, P., Bill, C., Shi, J., & Crain, S. (2019). The spontaneous eMERGEence of recursion in child language. Proceedings of BUCLD, 43, 270–285.Google Scholar
Gu, C. (2008). Structural ambiguity and AP?PP recursion in language acquisition. Ms., University of Massachusetts.Google Scholar
Hamburger, H., & Crain, S. (1984). Acquisition of cognitive compiling. Cognition, 17(2), 85–136. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoekstra, T. (1984). Transitivity: Grammatical relations in Government-Binding theory. Foris.Google Scholar
Hoeksema, J. (2010). Feature percolation in the Dutch possessive. In C. J. Zwart and M. de Vries (Eds.), Structure Preserved: Studies in syntax for Jan Koster (pp. 167-174). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hollebrandse, B. (2018). Indirect recursion: The importance of second-order embedding and its implications for cross-linguistic research. In L. Amaral, M. Maia, A. Nevins, & T. Roeper (Eds.), Recursion across domains, 35–47. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hollebrandse, B., & Roeper, T. (2014). Empirical results and formal approaches to recursion in acquisition. In T. Roeper & M. Speas (Eds.), Recursion: Complexity in cognition, studies in theoretical psycholinguistics (pp. 179–219). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hornstein, N., Rosen, S., & Uriagereka, J. (1994). Integrals. University of Maryland Working Papers in Linguistics, 2, 70–90.Google Scholar
Huang, C.-T. J., Li, Y. Audrey, & Li, Y. (2009). The syntax of Chinese. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (2010). Your theory of language evolution depends on your theory of language. In R. K. Larson, V. Déprez, & H. Yamakido (Eds.), The evolution of human language: Biolinguistic perspectives (pp. 63–72). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lasnik, H., & Lidz, J. (2017). The argument from the poverty of the stimulus. In I. Roberts (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of Universal Grammar (pp. 221–248). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Lieven, E. (2009). Developing constructions. Cognitive Linguistics, 20(1), 191–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Limbach, M., & Adone, D. (2010). Language acquisition of recursive possessives in English. Proceedings of BUCLD, 34, 281–290.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project. Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Marcus, G. (1993). Negative evidence in language acquisition. Cognition, 46(1), 53–85. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nevins, A., Pesetsky, D., & Rodrigues, C. (2009). Evidence and argumentation: A reply to Everett (2009). Language, 85(3), 671–681. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Castilla-Earls, A., Bejar, S., & Massam, D. (2012). Elmo’s sister’s ball: The problem of acquiring nominal recursion. Language Acquisition, 19(4), 301–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perfors, A., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Regier T. (2011). The learnability of abstract syntactic principles. Cognition, 118, 306–338. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: What’s special about it. Cognition, 95, 201–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richards, N. (2006). A distinctness condition on linearization. Ms., Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
(2010). Uttering trees. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roeper, T. (2007). The prism of grammar: How child language illuminates humanism. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010). Recursion: What is innate, why it needs a trigger, where it belongs in cross-linguistic work, and how it fits into the mind. Proceedings of Anpoll’s 1st International Psycholinguistics Congress, Rio de Janeiro (pp. 42–64).Google Scholar
(2011). The acquisition of recursion: How formalism articulates the child’s path. Biolinguistics, 5(1–2), 57–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roeper, T., & Snyder, W. (2005). Language learnability and the forms of recursion. In A.-M. DiScullo (Ed.), UG and external systems: Language, brain and computation (pp. 155–169). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ross, J. R. (1967). Constraints on variables in syntax (Unpublished PhD dissertation). Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
(1972). Doubl-ing. Linguistic Inquiry, 3, 61–86.Google Scholar
Sauerland, U. (2018). False reports in Pirahã. In L. Amaral, M. Maia, A. Nevins, & T. Roeper (Eds.), Recursion across domains (pp. 21–34). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
van Riemsdijk, H. C. (1988). The representation of syntactic categories. In Proceedings of the Conference on the Basque Language, Second Basque World Congress (pp. 104–116). Central Publications Service of the Basque Government.Google Scholar
(1998). Categorial feature magnetism: The endocentricity and distribution of projections. Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics, 2, 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wolfe, T. (2016). The kingdom of speech. Random House.Google Scholar
Yang, C., Crain, S., Berwick, R. C., Chomsky, N., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2017). The growth of language: Universal Grammar, experience, and principles of computation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 81, 103–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar