Markedness

Bernard Comrie
Table of contents

The intuition underlying the notion of markedness as used in contemporary linguistic theory is that, where a particular feature (in a broad, perhaps pretheoretic sense) has two (or more) values, one of these values is more usual, more expected, more natural than the other(s). This more usual value is called unmarked, the other value or values are called marked. (In the case of a feature with three or more values, it is conceivable that the different values could be ranged in order of markedness, from least to most marked.)

Full-text access is restricted to subscribers. Log in to obtain additional credentials. For subscription information see Subscription & Price.

References

Battistella, E.L.
1990Markedness: the evaluative superstructure of language. State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D.
1984‘The language bioprogram hypothesis’. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7: 173–221. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. & M. Halle
1968The sound pattern of English. Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J.H.
1966Language universals with special reference to feature hierarchies. Mouton.Google Scholar
Hyams, N.
1986Language acquisition and the theory of parameters. D. Reidel. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, R.
1968Child language aphasia and phonological universals. Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trubetzkoy, N.S.
1968Principles of phonology. Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar