Support and evidence for considering local contingencies in studying and transcribing silence in conversation

Israel Berger

Abstract

Using a conversation analytic methodology, this report looks at conversations in English in which lengthy silences are regularly present. These silences are treated as unproblematic in this corpus. They apparently deviate from the proposals that gaps are minimized (Sacks, Schegloff, & Jefferson 1974) and that there is a standard maximum silence of one second (Jefferson, 1988). This is discussed in light of context and culture. Then the robustness of some features of the organisation of sequences (Schegloff 2007) and turn- taking (Sacks, Schegloff, & Jefferson 1974) are considered. Finally, solutions are compared for rendering lengthy silences in such a way that their meaning is preserved in conversation analytic transcripts or others that include timed silences.

Keywords:
Quick links
A browser-friendly version of this article is not yet available. View PDF
Bateson, G
(1935) Culture contact and schismogenesis. Man 35: 178-183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dickerson, P., P. Stribling, and J. Rae
(2007) Tapping into interaction: How children with autistic spectrum disorders design and place tapping in relation to activities in progress. Gesture 7.3: 271-303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gardner, R., R. Fitzgerald, and I. Mushin
(2009) The underlying orderliness in turn-taking: Examples from Australian talk. Australian Journal of Communication 36.3: 65-90.Google Scholar
Giles, H., and J.M. Wiemann
(1987) Language, social comparison and power. In C.R. Berger and S.H. Chaffee (eds.), The Handbook of Communication Science. Newbury Park, CA: Sage, pp. 350-384.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C
(1979) The interactive construction of a sentence in natural conversation. In G. Psathas (ed.), Everyday Language: Studies in Ethnomethodology. New York: Irvington, pp. 97-121.Google Scholar
(2002) Conversational frameworks for the accomplishment of meaning in aphasia. In C. Goodwin (ed.), Conversation and Brain Damage. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 90-116.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Gourley, L.E
(1973) Capital B for Black. American Psychologist 30.2: 181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jefferson, G
(1988) Notes on a possible metric which provides for a 'standard maximum' silence of approximately one second in conversation. In D. Roger and P. Bull (eds.), Conversation: An interdisciplinary perspective. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters, pp. 167-196.Google Scholar
McWhorter, J
(2004, September 18) Call me Black. That's with a capital B. Not African-American. Houston Chronicle. Retrieved from http://​www​.chron​.com​/disp​/story​.mpl​/editorial​/outlook​/2802824​.htmlGoogle Scholar
Pomerantz, A.M
(1984) Agreeing and disagreeing with assessment: Some features of preferred/dis- preferred turn shapes. In J.M. Atkinson and J. Heritage (eds.), Structure of Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 57-101.Google Scholar
Prince, R
(1997) Is talk cheap or silence golden: The negotiation of activity in psychotherapy. Psychoanalytic Inquiry 17: 549-558. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Psathas, G., and T. Anderson
(1990) The 'practices' of transcription in conversation analysis. Semiotica 78: 75-99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sacks, H., E.A. Schegloff, and G. Jefferson
(1974) A simplest systematics for the organization of turn- taking for conversation. Language 50: 696-735. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, E.A
(2007) Sequence Organization in Interaction: A Primer in Conversation Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
Streeck, J
(2002) Grammars, words, and embodied meanings: On the uses and evolution of so and like. Journal of Communication 52: 581–596. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Street, R., and H. Giles
(1982) Speech accommodation theory: A social cognitive approach to language and speech behavior. In M. Roloff & C.R. Berger (eds.), Social Cognition and Communication. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage, pp. 193-226.Google Scholar
Stribling, P., J. Rae, and P. Dickerson
(2007) Two forms of spoken repetition in a girl with autism. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 42: 427–444. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tannen, D
(1984) The pragmatics of cross-cultural communication. Applied Linguistics 5: 189-195. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
This Wicked Day
(2010, June 16) Capitalisation is a political act. [Web log comment] Retrieved from http://​wickedday​.wordpress​.com​/2010​/06​/16​/capitalisation​-is​-a​-political​-act/ (2011, February 12)
Wilson, M., and T.P. Wilson
(2005) An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 12: 957-968. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, T.P., and D.H. Zimmerman
(1986) The structure of silence between turns in two-party conversation. Discourse Processes 9: 375-390. DOI logoGoogle Scholar