Part of
Gaze in Human-Robot Communication
Edited by Frank Broz, Hagen Lehmann, Bilge Mutlu and Yukiko Nakano
[Benjamins Current Topics 81] 2015
► pp. viixvi
References
Argyle, M
(1988) Bodily communication (2nd ed.). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, T
(1997) Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition, 4, 311–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., & Walker, J
(1995) Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 379–398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I
(2001) The ‘reading the mind in the eyes’ test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Broz, F., Lehmann, H., Nehaniv, C.L., & Dautenhahn, K
(2012) Mutual gaze, personality, and familiarity: Dual Eye-tracking during conversation. In Proceedings of IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (Ro-Man) .
Cassell, J., Sullivan, J., Prevost, S., & Churchill, E.F
(Eds.) (2000) Embodied conversational agents. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Farroni, T
(2003) Infants perceiving and acting on the eyes: Tests of an evolutionary hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(3), 199–212. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A
(1967) Some functions of gaze-direction in social interaction. Acta Psychologica, 26, 22–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kleinke, C
(1986) Gaze and eye contact: A research review. Psychological Bulletin, 100(1), 78–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kobayashi, H., & Kohshima, S
(1997) Unique morphology of the human eye. Nature, 387, 767–768. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001) Unique morphology of the human eye and its adaptive meaning: Comparative studies on external morphology of the primate eye. Journal of Human Evolution, 40, 419–435. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A.N., & Brooks, R
(2007) Eyes wide shut: The importance of eyes in infant gaze following and understanding other minds. In R. Flom, K. Lee, & D. Muir (Eds.), Gaze following: Its development and significance (pp. 217–241). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mutlu, B., Shiwa, T., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., & Hagita, N
(2009) Footing in human-robot conversations: How robots might shape participant roles using gaze cues. In Proceedings of the 4th ACM/IEEE Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI 2009) , San Diego, CA. DOI logo
Nakano, Y.I., Reinstein, G., Stocky, T., & Cassell, J
(2003) Towards a model of face-to-face grounding. In Proceedings of the 41st Annual Meeting on Association for Computational Linguistics-Volume 1 (pp. 553–561). Association for Computational Linguistics.
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., Lehmann, H., & Call, J
(2007) Reliance on head versus eyes in the gaz following of great apes and human infants: The cooperative eye hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution, 52, 314–320. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K.J
(2001) Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(01), 3–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yu, C., Schermerhorn, P., & Scheutz, M
(2012) Adaptive eye gaze patterns in interactions with human and artificial agents. ACM Transactions on Interactive Intelligent System, 1(2), 13: 1–13: 25.Google Scholar