Article published In:
Information Design Journal
Vol. 22:1 (2016) ► pp.318
References
Beier, S
(2012) Reading letters: designing for legibility. Amsterdam: BIS Publishers.Google Scholar
(2013) Legibility investigations: controlling typeface variables. Praxis and Poetics: Research through Design, Conference Proceedings (pp. 92–95). Newcastle upon Tyne and Gateshead.
Bringhurst, R
(1992) The elements of typographic style. Point Roberts, WA: Hartley & Marks Publishers.Google Scholar
Dyson, M.C
(2014) Applying psychological theory to typography: is how we perceive letterforms special? In D. Machin (Ed.), Visual Communication (pp. 215–242). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Frutiger, A
(1998) Signs and symbols: their design and meaning. London: Ebury Press.Google Scholar
Gauthier, I., Wong, A.C.-N., Hayward, W.G., & Cheung, O.S
(2006) Font tuning associated with expertise in letter perception. Perception, 351, 541–559. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haber, R.N., & Hershenson, M
(1980) The psychology of visual perception. New York, London: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Legge, G.E
(2007) Psychophysics of reading in normal and low vision. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Medler, D.A., & Binder, J.R
(2005) MCWord: An On-Line Orthographic Database of the English Language. [URL]
O’Brien, F., & Cousineau, D
(2014) Representing error bars in within-subject designs in typical software packages. The Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 10(1), 56–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K
(2007) Visual word recognition. In M.G. Gaskell (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 71–88). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sanocki, T
(1987) Visual knowledge underlying letter perception: font-specific schematic tuning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(2), 267–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1988) Font regularity constraints on the process of letter recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(3), 472–480. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1991a) Intrapattern and interpattern relations in letter recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 171, 924–941. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1991b) Looking for a structural network: effects of changing size and style on letter recognition. Perception, 20(4), 529–541. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tinker, M.A
(1965) Bases for effective reading. Minneapolis: Lund Press.Google Scholar
Walker, P
(2008) Font tuning: A review and new experimental evidence. Visual Cognition, 16(8), 1022–1058. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 8 other publications

Beier, Sofie & Chiron A.T. Oderkerk
2019. Smaller visual angles show greater benefit of letter boldness than larger visual angles. Acta Psychologica 199  pp. 102904 ff. DOI logo
Beier, Sofie, Chiron A. T. Oderkerk, Birte Bay & Michael Larsen
Bigelow, Charles
2019. Typeface features and legibility research. Vision Research 165  pp. 162 ff. DOI logo
Chow, Kenny K. N. & Kin Wai Michael Siu
2018. Proceedings of the 2018 ACM Conference Companion Publication on Designing Interactive Systems,  pp. 303 ff. DOI logo
Elbardawil, Shaima
2023. Empirical assessment of the legibility of the Naskh-style typeface used on Arabic road signs. Information Design Journal 28:2  pp. 178 ff. DOI logo
Lonsdale, Maria dos Santos, Xiaoxiao Qin, Yun Chen, Heather Green, Maha Algethami & Jiawen Xu
Oderkerk, Chiron A. T. & Sofie Beier
2022. Fonts of wider letter shapes improve letter recognition in parafovea and periphery. Ergonomics 65:5  pp. 753 ff. DOI logo
Thiessen, Myra, Sofie Beier & Hannah Keage
2020. A Review of the Cognitive Effects of Disfluent Typography on Functional Reading. The Design Journal 23:5  pp. 797 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 31 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.