Discourse level processing
This chapter provides an overview of how the visual-world eye-tracking paradigm has been used to investigate the processing and representation of discourse-level information. The chapter starts by reviewing some theoretical approaches to information structure, and then turns to visual-world experiments on the prosodic and syntactic reflexes of information structure, as well as the consequences of information structure for reference resolution. The notion of ‘prominence’ plays a central role in many of these investigations, in the shape of prosodic prominence (associated with new information), syntactic prominence (often associated with given or topical information) and representational prominence / accessibility in the domain of reference resolution. Comprehenders use prominence-related information to guide discourse-level aspects of processing, but prosodic prominence and syntactic prominence have different information-structural correlates. Furthermore, if we want to conceptualize reference resolution as a process sensitive to the prominence of mental representations, our view of what factors influence referential prominence needs to include not only entity-related factors (e.g. givenness), but also event-related factors (e.g. verb semantics and coherence relations between events). As a whole, the findings discussed in this chapter highlight the rapidity with which the human language processing system uses of discourse-level information, whether it be encoded in pitch accents, word order or the form of referring expressions. These findings suggest that discourse-level comprehension should not be relegated to a secondary stage of processing and instead occurs in tandem with other aspects of language comprehension, such as lexical access and syntactic processing.
References (113)
References
Allopenna, P.D., Magnuson, J.S., & Tanenhaus, M.K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419-439.
Almor, A. (1999). Noun-phrase anaphora and focus: The informational load hypothesis. Psychological Review, 106(4), 748-765.
Altmann, G.T.M. & Kamide, Y. 1999. Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73, 247-264.
Altmann, G.T.M. (2004). Language-mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: The ‘blank screen paradigm’. Cognition, 93, 79-87
Ariel, M. (1990). Accessing NP antecedents. London: Routledge, Croom Helm.
Ariel, M. (2001). Accessibility theory: An overview. In T. Sanders, J. Schilperoord, & W. Spooren (Eds.), Text representation, linguistic and psycholinguistic aspects (pp. 29-87). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Arnold, J.E., & Tanenhaus, M.K. (2011). Disfluency effects in comprehension: How new information can become accessible. In E. Gibson & N. Perlmutter (Eds.), The processing and acquisition of reference. MIT Press.
Arnold, J.E., Eisenband, J.G., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J.C. (2000). The immediate use of gender information: Eyetracking evidence of the time-course of pronoun resolution. Cognition, 76, B13-B26.
Arnold, J.E., Wasow, T., Losongco, A., & Ginstrom, R. (2000). Heaviness vs. newness: The effects of structural complexity and discourse status on constituent ordering. Language, 76, 28-55
Arnold, J.E., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J.C. (2007). Children's use of gender and order-of-mention during pronoun comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(4), 527-565.
Beckman, M.E. (1996). The parsing of prosody. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11, 17-67.
Beckman, M.E., & Ayers, G.M. (1997). Guidelines for ToBI labelling, vers 3.0. Ohio State University.
Birch, S.L., Albrecht, J.E., & Myers, J.L. (2000). Syntactic focusing structures influence discourse processing. Discourse Processes, 30, 285-304.
Birner, B., & Ward, G. (2009). Information structure and syntactic structure. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3/4, 1167-1187.
Bolinger, D. (1986). Intonation and its parts: Melody in spoken English. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Brennan, S.E., Friedman, M.A., & Pollard, C.J. (1987). A centering approach to pronouns. In
Proceedings of the 25th annual meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (pp. 155-162). Stanford, CA: Association for Computational Linguistics.
Brown-Schmidt, S. (2005) Language processing in conversation. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Rochester.
Brown-Schmidt, S., Byron, D.K., & Tanenhaus, M. (2005). Beyond salience: Interpretation of personal and demonstrative pronouns. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 292-313.
Büring, D. (1997). The meaning of topic and focus – The 59th Street Bridge accent. London: Routledge.
Chafe, W.L. (1974). Language and consciousness. Language, 50, 111-133.
Chafe, W.L. (1976). Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point of view. In C. Li (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp. 25-55). New York: Academic Press.
Chen, A., Den Os, E., & De Ruiter, J.P. (2007). Pitch accent type matters for online processing of information status: Evidence from natural and synthetic speech. The Linguistic Review, 24(2), 317-344.
Chomsky, N. (1971). Deep structure, surface structure, and semantic interpretation. In D. Steinberg & L. Jacobovits (Eds.), Semantics (pp. 183-216). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Clark, H.H., & Clark, E.V. (1977). Psychology and language. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Clark, H.H., & Haviland, S. (1977). Comprehension and the given-new contract. In R. Freedle (Ed.), Discourse production and comprehension (pp. 1-40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Clifton, C., & Frazier, L. (2004). Should given information come before new? Yes and no. Memory and Cognition, 32(6), 886-895.
Colonna, S., Schimke, S., & Hemforth, B. (2010). Le rôle de la structure informationnelle dans l’interprétation d’une anaphore pronominale inter-phrastique. In F. Neveu at al. (Eds.), Congrès Mondial de Linguistique Française, 1489-1499.
Cooper, R.M. (1974). The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84-107.
Cowles, H.W. (2003). Processing information structure: Evidence from comprehension and production. Ph.D. dissertation, UCSD.
Cowles, H.W., Walenski, M., & Kluender, R. (2007). Linguistic and cognitive prominence in anaphor resolution: Topic, constrastive focus and pronouns. Topoi, 26, 3-18.
Crawley, R.J., & Stevenson, R.J. (1990). Reference in single sentences and in texts. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 19(3), 191-210.
Cutler, A., & Fodor, J. (1979). Semantic focus and sentence comprehension. Cognition, 7, 49-59
Dahan, D., Tanenhaus, M.K., & Chambers, C.G. (2002). Accent and reference resolution in spoken-language comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 292-314.
Delin, J., & Oberlander, J. (1995). Syntactic constraints on discourse structure: The case of it-clefts. Linguistics, 33, 3.
Ellert, M., Järvikivi, J., & Roberts, L. (2009) Information structure guides gaze behavior: Processing the German subject pronouns er and der in spoken discourse. Poster presented at
15th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing
, Barcelona, Spain.
Engelhardt, P.E., Ferreira, F., & Patsenko, E.G. (2010). Pupillometry reveals processing load during spoken language comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 639-645.
Féry, C. Skopeteas, S., & Hörnig, R, . (2010). Cross-linguistic comparison of prosody, syntax and information structure in a production experiment on localizing expressions. Transactions of the Philological Society, 108(3), 329-351
Foraker, S., & McElree, B. (2007). The role of prominence in pronoun resolution: Active versus passive representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(3), 357-383
Garnham, A. (2001). Mental models and the interpretation of anaphora. Hove: Psychology Press.
Garvey, C., & Caramazza, A. (1974). Implicit causality in verbs. Linguistic Inquiry, 5, 459-464.
Gleitman, L., January, D., Nappa, R. & Trueswell, J. (2007). On the give and take between event apprehension and utterance formulation. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 544-569.
Gordon, P.C., Grosz, B.J., & Gilliom, L.A. (1993). Pronouns, names, and the centering o attention in discourse. Cognitive Science, 17, 311-347.
Griffin, Z.M., & Bock, J.K. (2000). What the eyes say about speaking. Psychological Science, 11, 274-279.
Gundel, J.K. (1974). The role of topic and comment in linguistic theory. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.
Gundel, J.K., & Fretheim, T. (2004). Topic and focus. In G. Ward & L. Horn (Eds.), Handbook of pragmatics (Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics). (pp.175-196). Oxford: Blackwell.
Gundel, J.K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R. (1993). Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language, 69, 274-307.
Gussenhoven, C. (1983). Focus, mode, and nucleus. Journal of Linguistics, 19, 377-417.
Halliday, M. (1967). Notes on transitivity and theme in English. Part 1 and 2. Journal of Linguistics, 3, 37-81; 199-244.
Hartshorne, Joshua K., Nappa, R., & Snedeker, J. (in press). Development of the first-mention bias. Journal of Child Language.
Haviland, S.E., & Clark, H.H. (1974). What's new? Acquiring new information as a process in comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 512-521.
Hedberg, N. (1990). Discourse pragmatics and cleft sentences in English. Ph.D dissertation, Universitiy of Minnesota.
Hedberg, N. (2000). The referential status of clefts. Language, 76, 891-920.
Hornby, P.A. (1974). Surface structure and presupposition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 530-538.
Huang, Y., & Snedeker, J. (2009). Online interpretation of scalar quantifiers: Insight into the semantics–pragmatics interface. Cognitive Psychology, 58(3), 376-415.
Hwang, Heeju, & Kaiser, Elsi. (2014). The role of the verb in grammatical function assignment in English and Korean. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 1363-1376.
Jackendoff, R. (1972). Semantic interpretation in generative grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Järvikivi, J., Van Gompel, R.P.G., Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J. (2005). Ambiguous pronoun resolution: Contrasting the first-mention and subject preference accounts. Psychological Science, 16, 260-264.
Kaiser, E., & Trueswell, J.C. (2008) Interpreting pronouns and demonstratives in Finnish: Evidence for a form-specific approach to reference resolution. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(5), 709-748.
Kaiser, E., & Trueswell, J.C. (2004) The role of discourse context in the processing of a flexible word-order language. Cognition, 94(2), 113-147.
Kaiser, E. (2009). Effects of anaphoric dependencies and semantic representations on pronoun interpretation. In S.L. Devi, A. Branco, & R. Mitkov (Eds.), Anaphora processing and applications (pp.121-130). Heidelberg: Springer.
Kaiser, E., Runner, J.T., Sussman, R.S., & Tanenhaus. M.K. (2009). Structural and semantic constraints on the resolution of pronouns and reflexives. Cognition, 112, 55-80.
Kaiser, Elsi. (2011a). Focusing on pronouns: Consequences of subjecthood, pronominalisation, and contrastive focus. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 1625-1666.
Kaiser, Elsi. (2011b). Salience and contrast effects in reference resolution: The interpretation of Dutch pronouns and demonstratives, Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 1587-1624.
Kamide, Y., Altmann, G.T.M., & Haywood, S. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye-movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 133-59.
Kehler, A. (2002). Coherence, reference, and the theory of grammar. Stanford: CSLI Publications.
Kehler, A., Kertz, L., Rohde, H., & Elman, J. (2008). Coherence and coreference revisited. Journal of Semantics (Special Issue on Processing Meaning), 25(1), 1-44.
Kiss, K.E. (1998). Identificational focus versus information focus. Language, 74, 245-273.
Knoeferle, P., Crocker, M.W., Scheepers, C., & Pickering, M.J. (2005). The influence of the immediate visual context on incremental thematic role assignment: Evidence from eye-movements in depicted events. Cognition, 95, 95-127.
Koornneef, A.W., & Van Berkum, J.J.A. (2006). On the use of verb-based implicit causality in sentence comprehension: Evidence from self-paced reading and eye tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 445-465.
Ladd, D.R. (1996). Intonational phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lambrecht, K. (1994). Information structure and sentence form: Topic, focus, and the mental representation of discourse referents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lambrecht, K. (2001). A framework for the analysis of cleft constructions. Linguistics, 39, 463-516.
Magnuson, J.S., Dixon, J.A., Tanenhaus, M.K., & Aslin, R.N. (2007). The dynamics of lexical competition during spoken word recognition. Cognitive Science, 31, 133-156.
Myhill, J. (1992). Typological discourse analysis. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Pierrehumbert, J., & Hirschberg, J. (1990). The meaning of intonational contours in the interpretation of discourse. In P.R. Cohen, J. Morgan, & M.E. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in communication (pp. 271-311). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Prince, E.F. (1978). A comparison of WH-clefts and IT-clefts in discourse. Language, 54, 883-906.
Pyykkönen, P., & Järvikivi, J. (2010). Activation and persistence of implicit causality information in spoken language comprehension. Experimental Psychology, 57 (1), 5-16.
Pyykkönen, P., Matthews, D., & Järvikivi, J. (2010). Three-year-olds are sensitive to semantic prominence during online language comprehension: A visual world study of pronoun resolution. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 115-129.
Reinhart, T. (1982). Pragmatics and linguistics: An analysis of sentence topics. University of Indiana Linguistics Club. (also Philosophica 1981, 27, 53-94).
Rooth, M. (1992). A Theory of focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics, 1, 75-116.
Schwarzschild, R. (1999). GIVENness, Avoid F and other constraints on the placement of focus. Natural Language Semantics, 7, 141-177.
Sedivy, J., Tanenhaus, M., Chambers, C., & Carlson, G. (1999). Achieving incremental semantic interpretation through contextual representation. Cognition, 71, 109-147.
Sekerina, I.E. & Trueswell, J.C. (2012). Interactive processing of contrastive expressions by Russian children. First Language 32: 63-87.
Selkirk, E.O. (1995). Sentence prosody: Intonation, stress, and phrasing. In J.A. Goldsmith (Ed.), The handbook of phonological theory (pp. 550-569). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Sgall, P., & Hajicova, W.E. (1977). Focus on focus. The Prague Bulletin of Mathematical Linguistics, 28: 5-54.
Song, H., & Fisher, C. (2005). Who’s ‘she’? Discourse prominence influences preschoolers comprehension of pronouns. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 29-57.
Spivey, M.J., Richardson, D.C., & Fitneva, S.A. (2004). Thinking outside the brain: Spatial indices to visual and linguistic Information. In J. Henderson & F. Ferreira (Eds.), Interfacing language, vision, and action (pp. 161-190). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Steedman, M. (2000). Information structure and the syntax–phonology interface. Linguistic Inquiry, 31, 649-689.
Strube, M., & Hahn, U. (1996). Functional centering. In
Proceedings of the 34th annual meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics
(pp. 270-277), Santa Cruz, CA.
Strube, M., & Hahn, U. (1999). Functional centering: Grounding referential coherence in information structure. Computational Linguistics, 25(3), 309-344.
Sturt, P., Sanford, A.J., Stewart, A., & Dawydiak, E. (2004). Linguistic focus and good-enough representations: An application of the change-detection paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 882-888.
Tanenhaus, M.K. (2007). Spoken language comprehension: insights from eye movements. In G. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 309-326). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Tanenhaus, M.K., Spivey-Knowlton, M., Eberhard, K.M., & Sedivy, J.C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632-1634.
Tavano, E., & Kaiser, E. (2008). Effects of stress and coherence on pronoun interpretation. Poster presented at the
21st Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing
, University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill.
Vallduví, E., & Vilkuna, M. (1998). On rheme and kontrast. In P. Culicover & M. Louise (Eds.), The limits of syntax. Syntax and semantics 29 (pp. 79-108). New York: Academic Press.
Vallduvi. E. (1990). The information component. Ph.D dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.
Van de Velde, M., Meyer, A.S., & Konopka, A.E. (2014). Message formulation and structural assembly: Describing "easy" and "hard" events with preferred and dispreferred syntactic structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 71(1), 124-144.
Venditti, J.J., Stone, M., Nanda, P., & Tepper, P. (2001). Discourse constraints on the interpretation of nuclear-accented pronouns. In
Proceedings of the 2002 International Conference on Speech Prosody
, Aix-en-Provence, France.
Vilkuna, M. (1989) Free word order in finnish: Its syntax and discourse functions. Helsinki: Finnish Literature Society.
Ward, G. (1985). The semantics and pragmatics of preposing. Ph.D dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.
Ward, P., & Sturt, P. (2007). Linguistic focus and memory: An eye-movement study. Memory and Cognition, 35, 73-86.
Weber, A., Braun, B., & Crocker, M.W. (2006). Finding referents in time: Eye-tracking evidence for the role of contrastive accents. Language and Speech, 49, 367-392.
Weber, A., Grice, M., & Crocker, M.W. (2006). The role of prosody in the interpretation of structural ambiguities: A study of anticipatory eyemovements. Cognition, 99, B63-B72.
Wilson. F. (2009). Processing at the syntax-discourse interface in second language acquisition. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Edinburgh.
Yee, E., Heller, D., & Sedivy, J.C. (2009). On the relationship between eye-movements and activation: Active vs. passive tasks during ambiguous pronoun resolution. Poster presented at the
22nd Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing
.
Zimmer, H.D., & Engelkamp, J. (1981). The given-new structure of cleft sentences and their influence on picture viewing. Psychological Research, 43, 375-389.
Cited by (1)
Cited by one other publication
Gračanin-Yuksek, Martina, Sol Lago, Duygu Fatma Şafak, Orhan Demir & Bilal Kırkıcı
2017.
The Interaction of Contextual and Syntactic Information in the Processing of Turkish Anaphors.
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 46:6
► pp. 1397 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 august 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.