Part of
Where do nouns come from?
Edited by John B. Haviland
[Benjamins Current Topics 70] 2015
► pp. 111133
References (30)
References
Aronoff, Mark, Irit Meir, Carol Padden, & Wendy Sandler (2004). Morphological universals and the sign langauge type. In Geert Booij & Jaap van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology (pp. 19–39). Berlin: Springer Netherland.Google Scholar
Boyatzis, Chris J. & Malcolm W. Watson (1993). Preschool children’s symbolic representation of objects through gestures. Child Development, 64, 729–735. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Butcher, Cynthia, Carolyn Mylander, & Susan Goldin-Meadow (1991). Displaced communication in a self-styled gesture system: Pointing at the nonpresent. Cognitive Development, 6, 315–342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feldman, Heidi M., Susan Goldin-Meadow, & Lila R. Gleitman (1978). Beyond Herodotus: The creation of language by linguistically deprived deaf children. In Andrew Lock (Ed.), Action, symbol, and gesture: The emergence of language (pp. 351–414). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Franklin, Amy, Anastasia Giannakidou, & Susan Goldin-Meadow (2011a). Negation as structure building in a home sign system. In Etsuyo Yuasa, Tista Bagchi, & Katharine Beals (Eds.), Pragmatics and autolexical grammar. In honor of Jerry Sadock (pp. 261–278). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Franklin, Amy, Giannakidou, Anastasia, & Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2011b). Negation, questions, and structure building in a home sign system. Cognition, 118 (3), 398–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Givon, Talmy (1979). On understanding grammar. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2003). The resilience of language: What gesture creating in deaf children can tell us about how all children learn language. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan & Carolyn Mylander (1984). Gestural communication in deaf children: The effects and noneffects of parental input on early language development. With commentaries by Jill de Villiers, Elizabeth Bates, & Virginia Volterra. And reply by the authors. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press ( Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 49 [3/4]).Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan, Cynthia Butcher, Carolyn Mylander, & Mark Dodge (1994). Nouns and verbs in a self-styled gesture system: What’s in a name? Cognitive Psychology, 27, 259–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan, Carolyn Mylander, & Cynthia Butcher (1995). The resilience of combinatorial structure at the word level: Morphology in self-styled gesture systems. Cognition, 56, 195–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan, David McNeill, & Jenny L. Singleton (1996). Silence is liberating: Removing the handcuffs on grammatical expression in the manual modality. Psychological Review, 103, 34–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodglass, Harold & Edith Kaplan (1963). Disturbance of gesture and pantomime in aphasia. Brain, 86 (4), 703–720. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, John A. (1988). Explaining language universals. In John A. Hawkins (Ed.), Explaining language universals (pp. 3–28). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul J. & Sandra A. Thompson (1984). The iconicity of the universal categories ‘noun’ and ‘verb’. In John Haiman (ed), Iconicity in syntax (pp. 151–183). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1988). The discourse basis for lexical categories in universal grammar. Language, 60 (4), 703–752. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hunsicker, Dea & Susan Goldin-Meadow (2012). Hierarchical structure in a self-created communication system: Building nominal constituents in homesign. Language, 88 (4), 732–763. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Reilly, Anne W. (1995). Using representations: Comprehension and production of actions with imagined objects. Child Development, 66, 999–1010. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Overton, Wilis F. & Joseph P. Jackson (1973). The representation of imagined objects in action sequences: A developmental study. Child Development, 44, 309–314. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Padden, Carol, Irit Meir, So-One Hwang, Ryan Lepic, Sharon Seegers, & Tory Sampson (2013). Patterned iconicity in sign language lexicons. Gesture, 13 (3), 287–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robins, Robert H. (1952). Noun and verb in universal grammar. Language, 28 (3), 289–298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sandler, Wendy, Irit Meir, Carol Padden, & Mark Aronoff (2005). The emergence of grammar: Systematic structure in a new language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America , 102, 2661–2665.
Sapir, Edward (1921). Language: An introduction to the study of speech. New York: Harcourt, Brace & Co.Google Scholar
Schachter, Paul (1985). Parts-of-speech systems. In Timothy Shopen (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description. Vol. 1: Clause structure (pp. 3–61). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Senghas, Ann (2005). Language emergence: Clues from a new Bedouin sign language. Current Biology, 15 (12), 463–465. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Supalla, Ted & Elissa L. Newport (1978). How many seats in a chair? The derivation of nouns and verbs in American sign language. In Patricia Siple (Ed.), Understanding language through sign language research (pp. 91–132). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, Sandra A. (1988). A discourse approach to the cross-linguistic category ‘adjective’. In John A. Hawkins (Ed.), Explaining language universals (pp. 167–185). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tkachman, Oksana & Wendy Sandler (2013). The noun-verb distinction in two young sign languages. Gesture, 13 (3), 253–286. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Waxman, Sandra R., Ann Senghas, & Susana Benveniste (1997). A cross-linguistic examination of the noun-category bias: Its existence and specificity in French- and Spanish-speaking preschool-aged children. Cognitive Psychology, 32, 183–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar