Part of
Phonological and Phonetic Considerations of Lexical Processing
Edited by Gonia Jarema and Gary Libben
[Benjamins Current Topics 80] 2015
► pp. 6786
References (28)
Berent, I., & Pinker, S. (2007). The dislike of regular plurals in compounds: Phonological or morphological. The Mental Lexicon, 2, 129–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernhardt, B.H., & Stemberger, J.P. (1998). Handbook of phonological development: From the perspective of constraint-based nonlinear phonology. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bleile, K.M. (1987). Regressions in the phonological development of two children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa.Google Scholar
Boersma, P., & Hayes, B. (2001). Empirical tests of the gradual learning algorithm. Linguistic Inquiry, 32, 45–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Browman, C.P., & Goldstein, L.M. (1986). Towards an articulatory phonology. Phonology Yearbook, 3, 219–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buck-Gengler, C.J., Menn, L., & Healy, A. (2004). What ‘‘mice-trap’’ tells us about the mental lexicon. Brain and Language, 90, 453–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fromkin, V.A. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language, 47, 27–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerken, L. (1991). The metrical basis for children’s subjectless sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 431–451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, M., & Crago, M.B. (1991). Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition, 39, 1–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, M., & Goad, H. (1997). What underlies inflectional error patterns in Genetic Dysphasia? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 10, 109–137. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, P. (1985). Level ordering in lexical development. Cognition, 21, 73–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haskell, T.R., MacDonald, M.C., & Seidenberg, M.S. (2003). Language learning and innateness: Some implications of compounds research. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 119–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hayes, B. (1999). Phonetically-driven phonology: The role of optimality theory and inductive grounding. InM. Darnell, E. Moravscik, M. Noonan, F. Newmeyer, & K. Wheatly(Eds.), Functionalism and formalism in linguistics, vol. 1 (pp. 243–285). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kehoe, M., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (1997). Truncation patterns in English-speaking children’s word productions. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 40, 526–541. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kiparsky, P. (1982). From cyclic phonology to lexical phonology. InH. van der Hulst & N. Smith(Eds.), The structure of phonological representations, part 1 (pp. 130–175). Dordrecht, Holland: Foris.Google Scholar
Klein, H.B., & Liu-Shea, M. (2009). Between-word simplification patterns in the continuous speech of children with speech sound disorders. Language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 40, 17–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. Third Edition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Marshall, C.R., & Van der Lely, H.K.J. (2006). A challenge to current models of past tense inflection: the impact of phonotactics. Cognition, 100, 302–320. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Menn, L., & Matthei, E. (1992). The “two-lexicon” approach of child phonology: Looking back, looking ahead. InC.A. Ferguson, L. Menn, & C. Stoel-Gammon(Eds.), Phonological development: Models, research, implications (pp. 211–248). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S., & Prince, A.S. (1988). On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S., & Ullman, M.T. (2002). The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 456–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prince, A., & Smolensky, P. (2004). Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramscar, M., & Dye, M. (2010). Learning language from the input: Why innate constraints can’t explain noun compounding. Cognitive Psychology, 62, 1–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smolensky, P. (1999). Grammar-based connectionist approaches to language. Cognitive Science, 23, 589–613. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stemberger, J.P. (1993). Glottal transparency. Phonology, 10, 107–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2007). Children’s overtensing errors: Phonological and lexical effects on syntax. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 49–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Storkel, H.L., Armbrüster, J., & Hogan, T.P. (2006). Differentiating phonotactic probability and neighborhood density in adult word learning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 1175–1192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Storkel, H.L., & Rogers, M.A. (2000). The effect of probabilistic phonotactics on lexical acquisition. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 14, 407–425. DOI logoGoogle Scholar