Part of
Prediction in Second Language Processing and Learning
Edited by Edith Kaan and Theres Grüter
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 12] 2021
► pp. 167186
References
Andringa, S.
(2020) The emergence of awareness in uninstructed L2 learning: A visual world eye tracking study. Second Language Research. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arai, M.
(2016) Importance of making (prediction) errors in L2 processing. Poster presented at the 22nd Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing, Bilbao, Spain, September 2016.
Atkinson, E., Wagers, M. W., Lidz, J., Phillips, C., & Omaki, A.
(2018) Developing incrementality in filler-gap dependency processing. Cognition, 179, 132–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. K.
(1986) Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 355–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L., & Fernald, A.
(2012) Knowing a lot for one’s age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(4), 417–436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brothers, T., Dave, S., Hoversten, L. J., Traxler, M. J., & Swaab, T. Y.
(2019) Flexible predictions during listening comprehension: Speaker reliability affects anticipatory processes. Neuropsychologia, 135, 107225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brothers, T., Swaab, T. Y., & Traxler, M. J.
(2017) Goals and strategies influence lexical prediction during sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 203–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N.
(2016) The now-or-never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, E62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chun, E.
(2020) L2 prediction guided by linguistic experience. English Teaching, 75, 79–103. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Felser, C.
(2006) Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(1), 3–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Some notes on the shallow structure hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(3), 693–706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Curcic, M., Andringa, S., & Kuiken, F.
(2019) The role of awareness and cognitive aptitudes in L2 predictive language processing. Language Learning, 69, 42–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dekydtspotter, L., & Renaud, C.
(2014) On second language processing and grammatical development: The parser in second language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4(2), 131–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., & Chang, F.
(2014) The P-chain: Relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634), 20120394. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkgraaf, A., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Duyck, W.
(2017) Predicting upcoming information in native-language and non-native-language auditory word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(5), 917–930. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, A. F. J., & Van Heuven, W. J.
(2002) The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5, 175–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Kroff, J. R. V., Tamargo, R. E. G., & Gerfen, C.
(2013) When gender and looking go hand in hand: Grammatical gender processing in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(2), 353–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A. L., & Marchman, V. A.
(2008) Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language. In I. A. Sekerina, E. M. Fernández, & H. Clahsen (Eds.), Developmental psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children’s language processing (pp. 97–135). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V. S., & Bock, K.
(2006) The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(7–8), 1011–1029. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fine, A. B., Jaeger, T. F., Farmer, T. A., & Qian, T.
(2013) Rapid expectation adaptation during syntactic comprehension. PloS one, 8(10), e77661. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. D.
(1999) Learnability theory: Triggers for parsing with. In E. C. Klein & G. Martohardjono (Eds.), The development of second language grammars: A generative approach (p. 373–406). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. D., & Inoue, A.
(1994) The diagnosis and cure of garden paths. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 23(5), 407–434. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C., Kim, S. K., Choo, H., Ghio, A., Herschensohn, J., & Koh, S.
(2019) Look and listen! The online processing of Korean case by native and non-native speakers. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(3), 385–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gambi, C., Pickering, M. J., & Rabagliati, H.
(2016) Beyond associations: Sensitivity to structure in preschoolers’ linguistic predictions. Cognition, 157, 340–351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) The relation between preschoolers’ vocabulary development and their ability to predict and recognize words. Child Development. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T., Rohde, H., & Schafer, A. J.
(2017) Coreference and discourse coherence in L2: The roles of grammatical aspect and referential form. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(2), 199–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T., Lau, E., & Ling, W.
(2020) How classifiers facilitate predictive processing in L1 and L2 Chinese: The role of semantic and grammatical cues. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(2), 221–234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hanulíková, A., Van Alphen, P. M., Van Goch, M. M., & Weber, A.
(2012) When one person’s mistake is another’s standard usage: The effect of foreign accent on syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(4), 878–887. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harrington Stack, C. M., James, A. N., & Watson, D. G.
(2018) A failure to replicate rapid syntactic adaptation in comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 46(6), 864–877. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartshorne, J. K., Nappa, R., & Snedeker, J.
(2015) Development of the first-mention bias. Journal of Child Language, 42(2), 423–446. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henry, N., Hopp, H., & Jackson, C. N.
(2020) Cue coalitions and additivity in predictive processing: the interaction between case and prosody in L2 German. Second Language Research. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H.
(2013) Grammatical gender in adult L2 acquisition: Relations between lexical and syntactic variability. Second Language Research, 29(1), 33–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Individual differences in the L2 processing of object-subject ambiguities. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(2), 129–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Semantics and morphosyntax in L2 predictive sentence processing. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 53(3), 277–306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Learning (not) to predict: Grammatical gender processing in adult L2 acquisition. Second Language Research, 32(2), 277–307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) The bilingual mental lexicon in L2 sentence processing. Second Language, 17, 5–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) Morphosyntactic adaptation in adult L2 processing: Exposure and the processing of case and tense violations. Applied Psycholinguistics, 41(3), 627–656. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H., & Lemmerth, N.
(2018) Lexical and syntactic congruency in L2 predictive gender processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(1), 171–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Guerra, E.
(2019) Effects of speech rate, preview time of visual context, and participant instructions reveal strong limits on prediction in language processing. Brain Research, 1706, 196–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & Janse, E.
(2016) Individual differences in working memory and processing speed predict anticipatory spoken language processing in the visual world. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 80–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Corley, M., & Pickering, M. J.
(2018) A cognitive load delays predictive eye movements similarly during L1 and L2 comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(2), 251–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C. N.
(2018) Second language structural priming: A critical review and directions for future research. Second Language Research, 34(4), 539–552. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C. N., & Hopp, H.
(2020) Prediction error and implicit learning in L1 and L2 syntactic priming. International Journal of Bilingualism, 24(5–6), 895–911. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C. N., & Ruf, H. T.
(2017) The priming of word order in second language German. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38(2), 315–345. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacob, G., & Felser, C.
(2016) Reanalysis and semantic persistence in native and non-native garden-path recovery. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(5), 907–925. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F., & Snider, N. E.
(2013) Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime’s prediction error given both prior and recent experience. Cognition, 127(1), 57–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jones, S. D., & Westermann, G.
(2020, May 15). Predictive processing and developmental language disorder. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaan, E.
(2014) Predictive sentence processing in L2 and L1: What is different? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4(2), 257–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Knowing without predicting, predicting without learning. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 5(4), 482–486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaan, E., & Chun, E.
(2018a) Priming and adaptation in native speakers and second-language learners. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(2), 228–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018b) Syntactic adaptation. In D. Watson & K. Federmeier (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 68, pp. 85–116). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kaan, E., Futch, C., Fuertes, R. F., Mujcinovic, S., & de la Fuente, E. Á.
(2019) Adaptation to syntactic structures in native and nonnative sentence comprehension. Applied Psycholinguistics, 40(1), 3–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kamide, Y., Scheepers, C., & Altmann, G. T.
(2003) Integration of syntactic and semantic information in predictive processing: Cross-linguistic evidence from German and English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(1), 37–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuperberg, G. R., & Jaeger, T. F.
(2016) What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 32–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lemhöfer, K., Schriefers, H., & Indefrey, P.
(2014) Idiosyncratic grammars: Syntactic processing in second language comprehension uses subjective feature representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(7), 1428–1444. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lemmerth, N. & Hopp, H.
(2019) Gender processing in simultaneous and successive bilingual children: Effects of lexical and syntactic cross-linguistic influence. Language Acquisition, 26(1), 21–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahowald, K., James, A., Futrell, R., & Gibson, E.
(2016) A meta-analysis of syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 5–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mani, N., & Huettig, F.
(2012) Prediction during language processing is a piece of cake – But only for skilled producers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(4), 843–847. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitsugi, S., & Macwhinney, B.
(2016) The use of case marking for predictive processing in second language Japanese. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 19–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morales, L., Paolieri, D., Dussias, P. E., & Valdés-Kroff, J.
(2016) The gender congruency effect during bilingual spoken-word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(2), 294–310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K.
(2020) Insights into the neural mechanisms of becoming bilingual: A brief synthesis of second language research with artificial linguistic systems. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(1), 87–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K. & Ullman, M. T.
to appear). Declarative and procedural memory in second language learning: Psycholinguistic considerations. In A. Godfroid & H. Hopp Eds. The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and psycholinguistics London Routledge
Myslín, M., & Levy, R.
(2016) Comprehension priming as rational expectation for repetition: Evidence from syntactic processing. Cognition, 147, 29–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nitschke, S., Serratrice, L., & Kidd, E.
(2014) The effect of linguistic nativeness on structural priming in comprehension. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(5), 525–542. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Omaki, A., & Lidz, J.
(2015) Linking parser development to acquisition of syntactic knowledge. Language Acquisition, 22(2), 158–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Phillips, C., & Ehrenhofer, L.
(2015) The role of language processing in language acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 5(4), 409–453. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Gambi, C.
(2018) Predicting while comprehending language: A theory and review. Psychological Bulletin, 144(10), 1002–1044. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S.
(2013) An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(04), 329–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1984) Language learnability and language development. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pozzan, L., & Trueswell, J. C.
(2015) Syntactic processing and acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 5(4), 516–521. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Second language processing and revision of garden-path sentences: A visual word study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(3), 636–643. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rabagliati, H., Gambi, C., & Pickering, M. J.
(2016) Learning to predict or predicting to learn? Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(1), 94–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramscar, M., Dye, M., & McCauley, S. M.
(2013) Error and expectation in language learning: The curious absence of” mouses” in adult speech. Language, 760–793. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, P.
(Ed.) (2015) Implicit and explicit learning of languages. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, P. & Williams, J.
to appear). The psycholinguistics of L2 learning: A cognitive psychology perspective. In A. Godfroid & H. Hopp Eds. The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and psycholinguistics London Routledge
Reuter, T., Borovsky, A., & Lew-Williams, C.
(2019) Predict and redirect: Prediction errors support children’s word learning. Developmental Psychology, 55(8), 1656–1665. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, L., & Liszka, S. A.
(2013) Processing tense/aspect-agreement violations on-line in the second language: A self-paced reading study with French and German L2 learners of English. Second Language Research, 29(4), 413–439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Safak, D. F. & Hopp, H.
2021). Verb bias and semantic persistence effects in L2 ambiguity resolution. Second Language Research. DOI logo
Shin, J. A., & Christianson, K.
(2012) Structural priming and second language learning. Language Learning, 62(3), 931–964. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2009) Constructing a language. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. M., & Logrip, M. L.
(1999) The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition, 73(2), 89–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Bergen, G., & Flecken, M.
(2017) Putting things in new places: Linguistic experience modulates the predictive power of placement verb semantics. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 26–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Gompel, R. P., & Arai, M.
(2018) Structural priming in bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(3), 448–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yan, S., & Jaeger, T. F.
(2020) Expectation adaptation during natural reading. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 1–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, C. D.
(2004) Universal Grammar, statistics or both? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(10), 451–456. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, C.
(2016) The price of linguistic productivity. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

Hopp, Holger
2022. Second Language Sentence Processing. Annual Review of Linguistics 8:1  pp. 235 ff. DOI logo
Kaan, Edith, Haoyun Dai & Xiaodong Xu
2023. Adaptation in L2 sentence processing: An EEG study. Second Language Research DOI logo
Schlenter, Judith
2023. Prediction in bilingual sentence processing: How prediction differs in a later learned language from a first language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 26:2  pp. 253 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 28 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.