References
Arias-Trejo, N., & Plunkett, K
(2009) Lexical–semantic priming effects during nfancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1536), 3633-3647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bain, B., & Yu, A
(1980) Cognitive consequences of raising children bilingually: “One Parent, One Language.” Canadian Journal of Psychology, 34, 304-313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bobb, S.C., & Mani, N
(2013) Categorizing with gender: Does implicit grammatical gender affect semantic processing in 24-month-old toddlers? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 297-308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & New, B
(2009) Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977-990. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & Van Heuven, W
(1998) The BIA model and bilingual word recognition. Localist Connectionist Approaches to Human Cognition, 189-225.Google Scholar
Elston-Güttler, K.E
. An inquiry into cross-language lexical-conceptual relationships and their effect on L2 lexical processing, PhD Dissertation, University of Cambridge 2000.
Elston-Güttler, K.E., & Gunter, T.C
(2009) Fine-tuned: Phonology and semantics affect first-to second-language zooming in. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 180-196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elston-Güttler, K.E., Gunter, T.C., & Kotz, S.A
(2005) Zooming into L2: Global language context and adjustment affect processing of interlingual homographs in sentences. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 57-70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P.S., Reznick, J.S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J.P., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J.S
(1993) The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Goldinger, S.D., Luce, P.A., Pisoni, D.B., & Marcario, J.K
(1992) Form-based priming in spoken word recognition: The roles of competition and bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R.M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Cauley, K.M., & Gordon, L
(1987) The eyes have it: Lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 14, 23-45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D.W., & Abutalebi, J
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 515-530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F
(1985) The bilingual as a competent but specific speaker-hearer. Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 6, 467-477. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1988) Exploring the recognition of guest words in bilingual speech. Language and Cognitive Processes, 3, 233-274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hermans, D., Bongaerts, T., De Bot, K., & Schreuder, R
(1998) Producing words in a foreign language: Can speakers prevent interference from their first language?. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 213-229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., & McQueen, J.M
(2007) The tug of war between phonological, semantic and shape information in language-mediated visual search. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 460-482. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ju, M., & Luce, P.A
(2004) Falling on sensitive ears constraints on bilingual lexical activation. Psychological Science, 15, 314-318. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J.F., & Bialystok, E
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 497-514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J.F., Bobb, S.C., & Wodniecka, Z
(2006) Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: Arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 119-135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lagrou, E., Hartsuiker, R.J., & Duyck, W
(2013) The influence of sentence context and accented speech on lexical access in second-language auditory word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 508-517. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luce, P.A., & Pisoni, D.B
(1998) Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19, 1–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mani, N. & Plunkett, K
(2008) Phonological priming in infancy. Proceedings of the 30 th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society , Washington, USA.
Mani, N., & Plunkett, K
(2010) In the infant’s mind’s ear evidence for implicit naming in 18-month-olds. Psychological Science, 21, 908-913. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Phonological priming and cohort effects in toddlers. Cognition, 121, 196-206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., & Spivey, M
(2003) Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within-and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 97-115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W.D., & Welsh, A
(1978) Processing interactions and lexical access during word recognition in continuous speech. Cognitive Psychology,10, 29-63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mayor, J., & Plunkett, K
(2014) Infant word recognition: Insights from TRACE simulations. Journal of Memory and Language, 71, 89-123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J.L., & Elman, J.L
(1986) The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1-86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meints, K., & Woodford, A
(2008) Lincoln Infant Lab Package 1.0: A new programme package for IPL, Preferential Listening, Habituation and Eyetracking. [www document: Computer software & manual]. [URL]
Morford, J.P., Wilkinson, E., Villwock, A., Piñar, P., & Kroll, J.F
(2011) When deaf signers read English: Do written words activate their sign translations?.Cognition, 118, 286-292. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Myers-Scotton, C
(2001) The matrix language frame model: Development and responses. Trends in Linguistics Studies and Monographs, 126, 23-58.Google Scholar
Paulmann, S., Elston-Güttler, K.E., Gunter, T.C., & Kotz, S.A
(2006) Is bilingual lexical access influenced by language context?. NeuroReport, 17, 727-731. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poarch, G.J., & Van Hell, J.G
(2012) Cross-language activation in children’s speech production: Evidence from second language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 419-438. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Quasthoff, U
(2002) DeutscherWortschatz im Internet [Online database]. Leipzig, Germany: University of Leipzig. [URL]Google Scholar
Radeau, M., Morais, J., & Segui, J
(1995) Phonological priming between monosyllabic spoken words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 1297–1311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shook, A., & Marian, V
(2013) The bilingual language interaction network for comprehension of speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(2), 304-324. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slowiaczek, L.M., & Hamburger, M
(1992) Prelexical facilitation and lexical interference in auditory word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Styles, S.J., Arias-Trejo, N., & Plunkett, K
(2008) Priming and interference in infancy. In Love, B.C., McRae, K. & Sloutsky V.M., (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society, 651-656.Google Scholar
Swingley, D., & Aslin, R.N
(2002) Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science, 13, 480-484. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Szagun, G., Stumper, B., & Schramm, S.A
(2009) Fragebogen zur frühkindlichen Sprachentwicklung (FRAKIS) und FRAKIS-K (Kurzform). Pearson.Google Scholar
Von Holzen, K. & Mani, N
(2012) Language nonselective lexical access in bilingual toddlers, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 569-586. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weber, A., & Cutler, A
(2004) Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 1-25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Bobb, Susan C. & Judith F. Kroll
2018. Chapter 14. Words on the brain. In Bilingual Cognition and Language [Studies in Bilingualism, 54],  pp. 307 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.