Chapter 1
Language control in the context of L3 acquisition
The centrality of individual differences
Current models of L3 syntactic transfer concern what is transferred during the initial stages. Each delineates a different, and singular, trajectory. From the outset though processes involved in co-opting available syntactic forms and pairing these with novel lexical items admit individual variability. Individual variability challenges the presumption of a single trajectory and so this chapter argues that understanding such variability is core to developing necessary and sufficient models of L3 acquisition. The chapter focuses on language control during L3 speech production as an important source of individual variability. Variability in language control affects speech production in L3 throughout the cycle from communicative intention to overt speech. What changes are needed to the processes of language control, and to the representations subject to their control, so that bilingual speakers may converse in an L3? Speakers must adapt their habits of language control so that they can construct and execute sentence plans in L3. Variability in the nature of such control will affect speech output. Current contexts of L3 use will also affect production in the two prior languages. Adaptive changes in language control induced via the acquisition and use of an L3 also impact on the production processes of the two prior languages. Models of L3 acquisition are ultimately models of multilingual processing.
Article outline
- Introduction
- Transfer as a probabilistic process
- Mechanisms of transfer: Representational change and use
- Sentence planning and execution in L3
- Review and discussion
- Conclusion
-
References
-
Appendix
References (63)
References
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2007). Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20(3), 242−275.
Argyropoulos, G. P., Tremblay, P., & Small, S. L. (2013). The neostriatum and response selection in overt sentence production: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 82, 53−60. .
Bardel, C., & Falk, Y. (2007). The role of the second language in third language acquisition: The case of Germanic syntax. Second Language Research, 23(4), 459−484. .
Bellander, M., Berggren, R., Mårtensson, J., Brehmer, Y., Wenger, E., Li, T-Q., Bodammer, N. C., Shing, Y-L., Werkle-Bergner, M., & Lövdén, M. (2016). Behavioral correlates of changes in hippocampal gray matter structure during acquisition of foreign vocabulary. NeuroImage, 131, 205–213. .
Bohland, J. W., Bullock, D., & Guenther, F. H. (2009). Neural representations and mechanisms for the performance of simple speech sequences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(7), 1504–1529. .
Cabrelli Amaro, J., Amaro, F., & Rothman, J. (2015). The relationship between L3 transfer and structural similarity across L3 development: Evidence from subject-to-subject raising over an experiencer. In H. Peukert (Ed.), Transfer effects in multilingual language development (Hamburg Studies on Linguistic Diversity 4) (pp. 21–52) Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Caffara, S., Monlinaro, N., Davidson, D., & Carreiras, M. (2015). Second language syntactic processing revealed through event-related potentials: an empirical review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 31–47. .
De Bot, K., & Jaensch, C. (2015). What is special about L3 processing? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 130–144. .
Dijkraaf, A., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Duyck, W. (2016). Predicting upcoming information in native-language and non-native-language auditory word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. .
Duñabeitia, J. A., Dimitropoulou, M., Dowens, M. G., Molinaro, N., & Martin, C. (2016). The electrophysiology of the bilingual brain. In R. R. Heredia, J. Altarriba, & A. B. Cieslicka (Eds.), Methods in bilingual reading comprehension research (pp. 265–312). New York, NY: Springer.
Dussias, P. E., & Sagarra, N. (2007). The effect of exposure on syntactic parsing in Spanish – English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(1), 101–116. .
Fallah, N., Jabbari, A., & Fazilatfar, A. M. (2016). Source(s) of syntactic CLI: The case of L3 acquisition of English possessives by Mazandarani – Persian bilinguals. Second Language Research, 32(2), 225–245. .
Festman, J. (2006). Trilingual language processing investigated by means of introspective verbalizing during speaking in three Languages. Journal of Social and Ecological Boundaries, 2(1), 117–147.
Festman, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Münte, T. (2010). Individual differences in control of language interference in late bilinguals are mainly related to general executive abilities. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 6(1), 5–17. .
Flynn, S., Foley, C., & Vinnitskaya, I. (2004). The Cumulative-Enhancement model for language acquisition: Comparing adults’ and children’s patterns of development in first, second and third language acquisition. International Journal of Multilingualism, 1(1), 3–17. .
Foucart, A., Martin, C. D., Moreno, E. M., & Costa, A. (2014). Can bilinguals see it coming? Word anticipation in L2 sentence reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(5), 1461–1469. .
Foucart, A., Ruiz-Tada, E., & Costa, A. (2016). Anticipation processes in L2 speech comprehension: evidence from ERPs and lexical recognition task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 213–219. .
González Alonso, J., & Rothman, J. (2016). Coming of age in L3 initial stages transfer models: deriving developmental predictions and looking towards the future. International Journal of Bilingualism. .
Green, D. W. (1986). Control, activation and resource: a framework and a model for the control of speech in bilinguals. Brain and Language, 27(2), 210–223. .
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 1(2), 67–81. .
Green, D. W. (2011). Language control in different contexts: The behavioural ecology of bilingual speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 103. .
Green, D. W. (2016). Trajectories to L3 proficiency. International Journal of Bilingualism. .
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. .
Green, D. W., & Wei, L. (2014). A control process model of code-switching. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(4), 499–511. .
Grosjean, F. (1998). Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issues. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 131–149. .
Grossberg, S. (1978). A theory of human memory: Self-organization and performance of sensory-motor codes, maps, and plans. In R. Rosen & F. Snell (Eds.), Progress in theoretical biology (pp. 233–374). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Psychological Science, 15(6), 409–414. .
Houghton, G. (1990). The problem of serial order: A neural network model of sequence learning and recall. In R. Dale, C. Mellish, & M. Zock (Eds.), Current research in natural language generation (pp. 287–319). London: Academic Press.
Hunsaker, M. R., & Kesner, R. P. (2013). The operation of pattern separation and pattern completion processes associated with different attributes or domains of memory. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(1), 36–58. .
Jessner, U. (2008). A DST model of multilingualism and the role of metalinguistic awareness. The Modern Languages Journal, 92(2), 270–283. .
Johnson, K. (2004). On the systematicity of language and thought. Journal of Philosophy, 101(3), 111–139. .
Kriete, T., Noelle, D. C., Cohen, J. D., & O’Reilly, R. C. (2013). Indirection and symbol-like processing in the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(41), 16390–16395. .
Kroll, J. F., & Gollan, T. H. (2014). Speech planning in two languages: What bilinguals tell us about language production. In V. Ferreira, M. Goldrick, & M. Miozzo (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Production (pp. 165–181). Oxford: OUP.
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., & Wodniecka, Z. (2006). Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: Arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9(2), 119–135. .
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bice, K., & Perrotti, L. (2015). Bilingualism, mind & brain. Annual Review of Linguistics, 1(1), 377–394. .
Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior: The Hixon symposium (pp. 112–136). New York, NY: Wiley and Sons.
Lesage, E., Morgan, B. E., Olson, A. C., Meyer, A. S., & Miall, R. C. (2012). Cerebellar rTMS disrupts predictive language processing. Current Biology, 22(18), R794–R795. .
Libben, M. R., & Titone, D. A. (2009). Bilingual lexical access in context: evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal Experimental Psychology: Learning. Memory & Cognition, 35(2), 381–390. .
MacWhinney, B. (2005). A unified model of language acquisition. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 49–67). Oxford: OUP.
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: within- and between language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(2), 97–115. .
Marien, P., Engelborghs, S., Fabbro, F., & De Deyn, P. P. (2001). The lateralized linguistic cerebellum: A review and a new hypothesis. Brain and Language, 79(3), 580–600. .
Morales, J., Gómez-Ariza, C. J., & Bajo, M. T. (2013). Dual mechanisms of cognitive control in bilinguals and monolinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 531–546. .
Muysken, P. (2000). Bilingual speech: A typology of codemixing. Cambridge: CUP.
Pickering, M., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 329–347. .
Pliatsikas, C., Johnstone, T., & Marinis, T. (2014). Grey matter volume in the cerebellum is related to the processing of grammatical rules in a second language: A structural voxel-based morphometry study. Cerebellum, 13(1), 55–63. .
Ripollés, P., Marco-Pallarés, J., Hielscher, U., Mestres-Missé, Tempelmann, Heinze, H-J., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., & Noesselt, T. (2014). The role of reward in word learning and its implications for language acquisition. Current Biology, 24(21), 2606–2611. .
Rothman, J. (2011). L3 syntactic transfer selectivity and typological determinacy: The Typological Primacy Model. Second Language Research, 27(1), 107–127. .
Rothman, J. (2015). Linguistic and cognitive motivations for the Typological Primacy Model of third language (L3) transfer: Timing of acquisition and proficiency considered. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 179–190. .
Rothman, J., Bañón, J. A., & Alonso, J. G. (2015). Neurolinguistic measures of typological effects in multilingual transfer: Introducing an ERP methodology. Frontiers in Psychology: Language Sciences, 6, 1087. .
Sanz, C., Park, H. I., & Lado, B. (2015). A functional approach to cross-linguistic influence in ab initio L3 acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 236–251. .
Silbert, L. J. , Honey, C. J., Simony, E., Poeppel, D., & Hasson, U. (2014). Coupled neural systems underlie the production and comprehension of naturalistic narrative speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(43), E4687–E4696. .
Silveri, M. C., Leggio, M. G., & Molinari, M. (1994). The cerebellum contributes to linguistic production: A case of agrammatism of speech following right hemicerebellar lesion. Neurology, 44(11), 2047–2050. .
Slabakova, R. (2016). The scalpel model of third language acquisition. International Journal of Bilingualism.
Tanner, D., Inoue, K., & Osterhout, L. (2014). Brain-based individual differences in online L2 grammatical comprehension. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(2), 277–293. .
Thierry, G., & Wu, Y. J. (2007). Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(30), 12530–12535. .
Tyson, B., Lantrip, A. C., & Roth, R. M. (2014). Cerebellar contributions to implicit learning and executive function. Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 179–217.
Ullman, M. T. (2001). The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: The Declarative/Procedural Model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4(2), 105–22. .
Van Gelder, T. (1990). Compositionality: A connectionist variation on a classical theme. Cognitive Science, 14(3), 355–384. .
Van Hell, J. G., & Dijkstra, T. (2002). Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(4), 780–789. .
Westergaard, M., Mitrofanova, N., Mykhaylyk, R., & Rodina, Y. (2016). Crosslinguistic influence in the acquisition of a third language: The linguistic proximity model. International Journal of Bilingualism. .
Cited by (2)
Cited by two other publications
Angelovska, Tanja & Angela Hahn
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 august 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.