Part of
The Internal Context of Bilingual Processing
John Truscott and Michael Sharwood Smith
[Bilingual Processing and Acquisition 8] 2019
► pp. 285320
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W.
(2007) Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 242–275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D.
(2008) Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 557–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: Neural adaptation and reserve. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 689–698. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Abutalebi, J., Guidi, L., Borsa, V., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P., & Weekes, B.
(2015) Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychologia, 69, 201–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C.
(2010) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80, 207–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alladi, S., Bak T., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A., Chaudhri, J., & Kaul, S.
(2013) Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 8, 1938–1944. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Allen, R. J., Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J.
(2014) Evidence for two attentional components in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 1499–1509. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Allott, N.
(2013) Relevance theory. In A. Capone, F. Lo Piparo, & M. Carapezza (Eds.), Perspectives on linguistic pragmatics (pp. 57–98). Cham: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Allport, A.
(1993) Attention and control: Have we been asking the wrong questions? A critical review of twenty-five years. In D. E. Meyer & S. Kornblum (Eds.), Attention and performance XIV: Synergies in experimental psychology, artificial intelligence, and cognitive neuroscience (pp. 182–218). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Altarriba, J.
(2013) Emotion, memory, and bilingualism. In J. Altarriba & R. Heredia (Eds.), Foundations of bilingual memory (pp. 85–203). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Altarriba, J., & Basnight-Brown, D. M.
(2011) The representation of emotion vs. emotion-laden words in English and Spanish in the Affective Simon Task. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 310–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ameel, E., Malt, B. C., Storms, G., & van Assche, F.
(2009) Semantic convergence in the bilingual lexicon. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 270–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ameel, E., Storms, G., Malt, B. C., & Sloman, S. A.
(2005) How bilinguals solve the naming problem. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 60–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R.
(1993) Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(1995) Cognitive psychology and its implications, 4th ed. New York, NY: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Antoniou, K., Grohmann, K. K., Kambanaros, M., & Katsos, N.
(2016) The effect of childhood bilectalism and multilingualism on executive control. Cognition, 149, 18–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aparicio, X., Heidlmayr, K., & Isel, F.
(2017) Inhibition efficiency in highly proficient bilinguals and simultaneous interpreters: Evidence from language switching and stroop tasks. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 46, 1427–1451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arêas da Luz Fontes, A., & Schwartz, A.
(2010) On a different plane: Cross-language effects on the conceptual representations of within-language homonyms. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 508–532. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Working memory influences on cross-language activation during bilingual lexical disambiguation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 360–370. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arêas da Luz Fontes, A., Yeh, L., & Schwartz, A.
(2010) Bilingual lexical disambiguation: The nature of cross-language activation effects. Letrônica, 3(1), 107–128.Google Scholar
Ariely, D.
(2009) Predictably irrational: The hidden forces that shape our decisions (revised and expanded edition). New York, NY: Harper.Google Scholar
Aru, J., Aru, J., Priesemann, V., Wibral, M., Lana, L., Pipa, G., Singer, W., & Vicente, R.
(2015) Untangling cross-frequency coupling in neuroscience. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 31, 51–61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Athanasopoulos, P.
(2015) Conceptual representation in bilinguals: The role of language specificity and conceptual change. In J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing (pp. 275–292). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Auer, P.
(1984) Bilingual conversation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995) The pragmatics of code-switching: A sequential approach. In L. Milroy & P. Muysken (Eds.), One speaker two languages: Cross-disciplinary perspectives on code-switching (pp. 115–135). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baars, B.
(1988) A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(1997) Some essential differences between consciousness and attention, perception, and working memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 363–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baars, B. J., Fehling, M. R., LaPolla, M., & McGovern, K.
(1997) Consciousness creates access: Conscious goal images recruit unconscious action routines, but goal competition serves to “liberate” such routines, causing predictable slips. In J. D. Cohen & J. W. Schooler (Eds.), Scientific approaches to consciousness (pp. 432–444). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J., Ramsøy, T. Z., & Laureys, S.
(2003) Brain, conscious experience and the observing self. Trends in Neurosciences, 26, 671–675. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.
(1986) Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(1996) Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 5–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G.
(1974) Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Banich, M. T., & Depue, B. E.
(2015) Recent advances in understanding neural systems that support inhibitory control. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 1, 17–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barak, O., & Tsodyks, M.
(2014) Working models of working memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 25, 20–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barak, O., Tsodyks, M., & Romo, R.
(2010) Neuronal population coding of parametric working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 9424–9430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, K.
(1999) Exploring the interlanguage of interlanguage pragmatics: A research agenda for acquisitional pragmatics. Language Learning, 49, 677–713. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A.
(1997) The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition. Vol. 10. The automaticity of everyday life (pp. 1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bargh, J.
(2017) Before you know it: The unconscious reasons for what we do. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J.
(Eds.) (1992) The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C., & Kurzban, R.
(2006) Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate. Psychological Review, 113, 628–647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F.
(2005) Feeling is perceiving: Core affect and conceptualization in the experience of emotion. In L. F. Barrett, P. M. Niedenthal, and P. Winkielman (Eds.), Emotion and consciousness (pp. 255–284). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
(2017) How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. Boston, MA: Houghton, Mifflin, Harcourt.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Niedenthal, P. M., & Winkielman, P.
(Eds.) (2005) Emotion and consciousness. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Russell, J. A.
(1999) The structure of current affect: Controversies and emerging consensus. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 10–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L.
(1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Brain and Behavioural Sciences, 22, 577–660.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, W. K., Barbey, A. K., & Wilson, C. D.
(2003) Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 84–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bartolotti, J., & Marian, V.
(2013) Bilingual memory: Structure, access, and processing. In J. Altarriba & L. Isurin (Eds.), Memory, language, and bilingualism: Theoretical and applied approaches (pp. 7–47). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bastos, A. M., Vezoli, J., & Fries, P.
(2015) Communication through coherence with inter-areal delays. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 31, 173–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bauer, E. P., Paz, R., & Paré, D.
(2007) Gamma oscillations coordinate amygdalo-rhinal interactions during learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 9369–9379. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., & Vohs, K. D.
(2007) Self-regulation and the executive function: The self as controlling agent. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 516–539). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., & Tierney, J.
(2011) Willpower: Rediscovering the greatest human strength. New York, NY: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Behrens, H.
(2009) Usage-based and emergentist approaches to language acquisition. Linguistics, 37, 383–411.Google Scholar
Belazi, H. M., Rubin, E. J., & Toribio, A. J.
(1994) Code switching and X-Bar Theory: The Functional Head Constraint. Linguistic Inquiry, 25, 221–237.Google Scholar
Belletti, A., Bennati, E., & Sorace, A.
(2007) Theoretical and developmental issues in the syntax of subjects: Evidence from near-native Italian. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 25, 657–689. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bertolero, M. A., Yeo, B. T. T., & D’Esposito, M.
(2015) The modular and integrative functional architecture of the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 112, E6798–E6807. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bhatia, T. K., & Ritchie, W. C.
(Eds.) (2013) The handbook of bilingualism and multilingualism, 2nd ed. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bhatt, R. M., & Bolonyai, A.
(2011) Code-switching and the optimal grammar of bilingual language use. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 522–546. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(1990) Communication strategies: A psychological analysis of second-language use. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
(1993) Symbolic representation and attentional control in pragmatic competence. In G. Kasper & S. Blum-Kulka (Eds.), Interlanguage pragmatics (pp. 43–59). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. M.
(2010) Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 19–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G.
(2012) Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 240–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Poarch, G., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M.
(2014) Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29, 696–705. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Ryan, E. B.
(1985) Towards a definition of metalinguistic skill. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 31, 229–251.Google Scholar
Binder, J. R., & Desai, R. H.
(2011) The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 527–536. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blanco-Elorrieta, E., & Pylkkänen, L.
(2017) Bilingual language switching in the laboratory versus in the wild: The spatiotemporal dynamics of adaptive language control. Journal of Neuroscience, 37, 9022–9036. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blom, J.-P., & Gumperz, J.
(1972) Social meaning in linguistic structures: Code switching in northern Norway. In J. Gumperz & D. Hymes (Eds.), Directions in sociolinguistics: The ethnography of communication (pp. 407–434). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.Google Scholar
Bobb, S. C., & Wodniecka, Z.
(2013) Language switching in picture naming: What asymmetric switch costs (do not) tell us about inhibition in bilingual speech planning. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 568–585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L.
(2001) Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bressler, S. L., & Richter, C. G.
(2015) Interareal oscillatory synchronization in top-down neocortical processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 31, 62–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bridgett, D. J., Oddi, K. B., Laake, L. M., Murdock, K. W., & Bachmann, M. N.
(2013) Integrating and differentiating aspects of self-regulation: Effortful control, executive functioning, and links to negative affectivity. Emotion, 13, 47–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, G., & Yule, G.
(1983) Discourse analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruni, L., & Giorgi, G.
(2015) Towards a heterarchical approach to biology and cognition. Progress in Biophysics and Molecular Biology, 119, 481–492. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruya, B.
(Ed.) (2010) Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & Duyck, W.
(2010) Is it time to leave behind the Revised Hierarchical Model of bilingual language processing after fifteen years of service? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 359–371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bullock, B. E., & Toribio, A. J.
(2009) The handbook of linguistic code-switching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bultena, S., Dijkstra, T., & van Hell, J. G.
(2015) Language switch costs in sentence comprehension depend on language dominance: Evidence from self-paced reading. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 453–469. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, J., Flynn, S., & Rothman, J.
(Eds.) (2012) Third language acquisition in adulthood. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Canale, M., & Swain, M.
(1980) Theoretical bases of communicative approaches to second language teaching and testing. Applied Linguistics, 1, 1–47. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Canavier, C. C.
(2015) Phase-resetting as a tool of information transmission. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 31, 206–213. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Canolty, R. T., Edwards, E., Dalal, S. S., Soltani, M., Nagarajan, S. S., Kirsch, H. E., Berger, M. S., Barbaro, N. M., & Knight, R. T.
(2006) High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science, 313, 1626–1628. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cantor, J., & Engle, R. W.
(1993) Working-memory capacity as long-term memory activation: An individual-differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1101–1114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capa, R. L., Bustin, G. M., Cleeremans, A., & Hansenne, M.
(2011) Conscious and unconscious reward cues can affect a critical component of executive control: (un)conscious updating? Experimental Psychology, 58, 370–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D.
(1992) Language: Structure, processing and disorder. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Caramazza, A., & Mahon, B. Z.
(2003) The organization of conceptual knowledge: The evidence from category-specific semantic deficits. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 354–361. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carroll, S. E.
(1999) Putting ‘input’ in its proper place. Second Language Research, 15, 337–388. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carroll, S.
Carruthers, P.
(2006) The architecture of the mind: Massive modularity and the flexibility of thought. Oxford: Clarendon. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carter, R.
(2008) The people you are. London: Abacus.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F.
(1998) On the self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Castro, T., Rothman, J., & Westergaard, M.
(2017) On the directionality of cross-linguistic effects in bidialectal bilingualism. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1382, 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cavanagh, J. F., & Frank, M. J.
(2014) Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 414–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chamorro, G., Sturt, P., & Sorace, A.
(2016) Differential object marking in Spanish near-native speakers of English. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 45, 697–715. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Charland, L. C.
(2005) Emotion experience and the indeterminacy of valence. In L. F. Barrett, P. M. Niedenthal, & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Emotion and consciousness (pp. 231–254). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Chekaf, M., Cowan, N., & Mathy, F.
(2016) Chunk formation in immediate memory and how it relates to data compression. Cognition, 155, 96–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, H., Swan, G., & Wyble, B.
(2016) Prolonged focal attention without binding: Tracking a ball for half a minute without remembering its color. Cognition, 147, 144–148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1965) Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2000) New horizons in the study of language and mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christiansen, M., & Chater, N.
(2016) Creating language: Integrating evolution, acquisition and processing. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christoffels, I. K., & De Groot A. M. B.
(2004) Components of simultaneous interpreting: Comparing interpreting with shadowing and paraphrasing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 227–240. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B.
(2011) A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 73–101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cicourel, A.
(2006) The interaction of discourse, cognition and culture. Discourse Studies, 8, 25–29. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clayton, N. S., Salwiczek, L. H., & Dickinson, A.
(2007) Episodic memory. Current Biology, 17, R189–R191. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cockcroft, K., Wigdorowitz, M., & Liversage, L.
(2019) A multilingual advantage in the components of working memory. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22, 15–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cohen, N., Henik, A., & Moyal, N.
(2012) Executive control attenuates emotional effects – for high reappraisers only? Emotion, 12, 970–979. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A.
(2004) Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 75, 317–333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colgin, L., & Moser, E. I.
(2010) Gamma oscillations in the hippocampus. Physiology, 25, 319–329. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R.
(1969) Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Conway, M. A.
(2005) Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 594–628. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cook, V.
(2002) Background to the L2 user. In V. Cook (Ed.), Portraits of the L2 user (pp. 1–28). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cook, V., Bassetti, B., Kasai, C., Sasaki, M., & Takahashi, J.
(2006) Do bilinguals have different concepts? The case of shape and material in Japanese L2 users of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 10, 137–152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corder, S. P.
(1967) The significance of learners’ errors. IRAL, 5, 161–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J.
(2013) Evolutionary psychology: New perspectives on cognition and motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 201–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., & Caramazza, A.
(1999) Is lexical selection in bilingual speech production language-specific? Further evidence from Spanish-English and English-Spanish bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 231–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Santesteban, M., & Ivanova, I.
(2006) How do highly proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? Inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both functional. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 3, 1057–1074. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N.
(1993) Activation, attention, and short-term memory. Memory and Cognition, 21, 162–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000) The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Working memory capacity. New York, NY: Psychology Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N., Saults, J. S., & Blume, C. L.
(2014) Central and peripheral components of working memory storage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 1806–1836. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Craig, A. D.
(2002) How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 655–666. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003) Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 500–505. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) How do you feel – now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 59–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) The sentient self. Brain structure and function, 214, 563–577. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., Bialystok, E., & Freedman, M.
(2010) Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease. Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75, 1726–1729. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Csizér, K., & Magid, M.
(Eds.) (2014) The impact of self-concept on language learning. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A. R.
(1994) Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. London: Papermac.Google Scholar
(1999) The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
(2000) A second chance for emotion. In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 12–23). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A.
(2003) Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain. Orlando, FL: Harcourt.Google Scholar
(2010) Self comes to mind: Constructing the conscious mind. New York, NY: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Damasio, A., & Damasio, H.
(1993) Brain and language. Mind and brain. Readings from Scientific American Magazine (pp. 54–65). New York, NY: Freeman and Company.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H.
(1998) Somatic markers and the guidance of behavior. In J. M. Jenkins, K. Oatley, & N. L. Stein (Eds.), Human emotions: A reader (pp. 122–135). Malden, MA: Blackwell (reprint).Google Scholar
Davidson, R. J., & Irwin, W.
(1999) The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 11–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Bruin, A., Samuel, A. G., & Duñabeitia, J. A.
(2018) Voluntary language switching: When and why do bilinguals switch between their languages? Journal of Memory and Language, 103, 28–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Degani, T., & Tokowicz, N.
(2010) Semantic ambiguity within and across languages: An integrative review. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1266–1303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Groot, A. M. B.
(2013) Reading. In F. Grosjean & P. Li (Eds.), The psycholinguistics of bilingualism (pp. 73–99). Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
de Groot, A. M. B., & Starreveld, P. A.
(2015) Parallel language activation in bilinguals’ word production and its modulating factors: A review and computer simulations. In J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing (pp. 389–415). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S.
(2011) The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. Revised and updated edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L.
(2001) Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79, 1–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Leeuw, E.
(2014) Maturational constraints in bilingual speech. In E. M. Thomas & I. Mennen (Eds.), Advances in the study of bilingualism (pp. 25–37). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., & Chang, F.
(2014) The P-chain: Relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369, 1–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., & O’Seaghdha, P. G.
(1994) Inhibition in interactive activation models of linguistic selection and sequencing. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 409–453). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
de Mey, M.
(1977) The cognitive viewpoint: Its development and its scope. In M. De Mey, R. Pinxten, M. Poriau, & F. Vandamme (Eds.), C77 International workshop on the cognitive viewpoint (pp xvi–xxxi). Ghent: University of Ghent.Google Scholar
Dere, E., Kart-Teke, E., Huston, J. P., & De Souza Silva, M. A.
(2006) The case for episodic memory in animals. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 1206–1224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Desimone, R., & Duncan, J.
(1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
D’Esposito, M., & Postle, B. R.
(2015) The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 115–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dewaele, J-M.
(2010) Emotions in multiple languages. Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A.
(2013) Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dias, P., Villameriel, S., Giezen, M. R., Costello, B., & Carreiras, M.
(2017) Language switching across modalities: Evidence from bimodal bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43, 1828–1834.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T., & van Heuven, W. J. B.
(1998) The BIA model and bilingual word recognition. In J. Grainger & A. M. Jacobs (Eds.), Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition (pp. 189–225) Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2002) The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: From identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5, 175–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dijkstra, T., van Jaarsveld, H., & Ten Brinke, S.
(1998) Interlingual homograph recognition: Effects of task demands and language intermixing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 51–66. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Dik, S. C.
(1978) Functional grammar. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Di Sciullo, A.-M., Muysken, P., & Singh, R.
(1986) Government and code-mixing. Journal of Linguistics, 22, 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dolan, R. J.
(2002) Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science, 298, 1191–1194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Domenech, P., & Koechlin, E.
(2015) Executive control and decision-making in the prefrontal cortex. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 1, 101–106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dong, Y., Gui, S., & MacWhinney, B.
(2005) Shared and separate meanings in the bilingual lexical memory. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 8, 221–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dörnyei, Z.
(2009) The L2 motivational self system. In Z. Dörnyei & E. Ushioda (Eds.), Motivation, language identity and the L2 self (pp. 9–42). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dörnyei, Z., & Csizér, K.
(2002) Some dynamics of language attitudes and motivation: Results of a longitudinal nationwide survey. Applied Linguistics, 23, 421–462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dörnyei, Z., & Ushioda, E.
(2009) Motivation, language identities, and the L2 self: Future research directions. In Z. Dörnyei & E. Ushioda (Eds.), Motivation, language identity and the L2 self (pp. 350–356). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Driver, J., Davis, G., Russell, C., Turatto, M., & Freeman, E.
(2001) Segmentation, attention and phenomenal visual objects. Cognition, 80, 61–95.Google Scholar
Dulay, H., & Burt, M.
(1977) Remarks on creativity in language acquisition. In M. Burt, H. Dulay, & M. Finnochiaro (Eds.), Viewpoints on English as a second language (pp. 95–126). New York, NY: Regents.Google Scholar
Dupré, J.
(Ed.) (1987) The latest on the best: Essays on evolution and optimality. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dussias, P., Dietrich, A. J., & Villegas, Á.
(2015) Cross-language interactions during bilingual sentence processing. In J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing (pp. 349–366). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Edelman, G. M.
(1992) Bright air, brilliant fire: On the matter of the mind. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
(2004) Wider than the sky: The phenomenal gift of consciousness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
(2006a) The embodiment of mind. Daedalus, 135(3), 23–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006b) Second nature: Brain science and human knowledge. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H.
(2003) The hippocampus, episodic memory, declarative memory, spatial memory… where does it all come together? International Congress Series, 1250, 235–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P.
(2003) Emotions revealed: Understanding faces and feelings. London: Phoenix.Google Scholar
Elman, J.
(1995) Language as a dynamical system. In R. F. Port & T. van Gelder (Eds.), Mind as motion: Explorations in the dynamics of cognition (pp. 195–223). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Borinstein, H. B., Thompson, R., & Gollan, T. H.
(2008) Bimodal bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 43–61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, K., Petrich, J., & Gollan, T. H.
(2012) Bilingual processing of ASL-English code-blends: The consequences of accessing two lexical representations simultaneously. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 199–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Emonds, J.
(2000) Lexicon and grammar: The English syntacticon. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Engel, A. K., Fries, P., & Singer, W.
(2001) Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 704–716. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W., Cantor, J., & Carullo, J. J.
(1992) Individual differences in working memory and comprehension: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 972–992. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ervin, S. M.
(1964) Language and TAT content in bilinguals. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 500–507. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eubank, L.
(1994) Optionality and the initial state in L2 development. In T. Hoekstra & B. D. Schwartz (Eds.), Language acquisition studies in generative grammar (pp. 369–388). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996) Negation in early German-English interlanguage: More valueless features. Second Language Research, 12, 73–106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eubank, L., & Grace, S. T.
(1998) V-to-I and inflection in non-native grammars. In M.-L. Beck (Ed.), Morphology and its interfaces in second language knowledge (pp. 69–88). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, D.
(2001) Emotion: A very short introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2004) The search hypothesis of emotion. In D. Evans & P. Cruse (Eds.), Emotion, evolution, and rationality (pp. 179–191). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, D., & Cruse, P.
(Eds.) (2004) Emotion, evolution, and rationality. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, T. E., Adams, H. H. H., Licher, S., Wolters, F. J., van der Lugt, A., Ikram, M. K., Sullivan, M. J., Vernooij, M. W., & Ikram, M. A.
(2018) Subregional volumes of the hippocampus in relation to cognitive function and risk of dementia. NeuroImage, 178, 129–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Færch, C., & Kasper, G.
(1983) Strategies in interlanguage communication. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Feather, N. T.
(1990) Bridging the gap between values and actions. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior. Volume 2 (pp. 151–192). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ferguson, M. J.
(2008) On becoming ready to pursue a goal you don’t know you have: Effects of nonconscious goals on evaluative readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1268–1294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferstl, E.
(2006) The functional neuroanatomy of text comprehension. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences.Google Scholar
Ferstl, E. C.
(2007) The functional neuroanatomy of text comprehension: What’s the story so far? In F. Schmalhofer & C. Perfetti (Eds.), Higher level language processes in the brain: Inference and comprehension processes (pp. 53–102). Mahwah: NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2010) The neuroanatomy of discourse comprehension: Where are we now? In V. Bambini (Ed.), Neuropragmatics, Special Issue of Italian Journal of Linguistics, 22, 61–88.Google Scholar
Ferstl, E. C., Neumann, J., Bogler, C., & von Cramon, D. Y.
(2008) The extended language network: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 29, 581–593. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fichtenholtz, H. M., & LaBar, K. S.
(2012) Emotional influences on visuospatial attention. In G. R. Mangun (Ed.), The neuroscience of attention: Attentional control and selection. Oxford Scholarship Online. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fiebach, C. J., Friederici, A. D., Smith, E. E., & Swinney, D.
(2007) Lateral inferotemporal cortex maintains conceptual–semantic representations in verbal working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 2035–2049. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, C.
(1977) Scenes-and-frames semantics. In A. Zampolli (Ed.), Linguistic structures processing (pp. 55–81). Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
(2006) Frames semantics. In D. Geeraerts (Ed.), Cognitive linguistics: Basic readings (pp. 373–400). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, C., Kay, P., & Sag, I.
(2005) Construction grammar. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Flynn, S., Foley, C., & Vinnitskaya, I.
(2004) The Cumulative-Enhancement Model of language acquisition: Comparing adults’ and children’s patterns of development in first, second and third language acquisition of relative clauses. International Journal of Multilingualism, 1, 3–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor J. A.
(1983) The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foster, B. L., Kaveh, A., Dastjerdi, M., Miller, K. J., & Parvizi, J.
(2013) Human retrosplenial cortex displays transient theta phase locking with medial temporal cortex prior to activation during autobiographical memory retrieval. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 10439–10446. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foster-Cohen, S. H., Sharwood Smith, M., Sorace, A., & Ota, M.
(Eds.) (2004) EUROSLA Yearbook, Vol. 4. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
French, L. M.
(2003) Phonological working memory and L2 acquisition: A developmental study of Quebec Francophone children learning English. Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Université Laval, Quebec.Google Scholar
Friederici, A. D., & Gierhan, S. M. E.
(2012) The language network. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 250–254. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fries, P.
(2005) A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: Neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 474–480. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Neuronal gamma-band synchronization as a fundamental process in cortical computation. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 209–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friesen, D. C., & Bialystok, E.
(2012) Metalinguistic ability in bilingual children: The role of executive control. Rivista Di Psicolinguistica Applicata, 12, 47–56. PMID 24782696 Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H.
(1986) The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(1988/1998) The laws of emotion. In J. M. Jenkins, K. Oatley, & N. L. Stein (Eds.), Human emotions: A reader (pp. 270–287). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Fusi, S.
(2008) A quiescent working memory. Science, 319, 1495–1496. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuster, J. M.
(1995) Memory in the cerebral cortex. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2015) The prefrontal cortex, 5th ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Gao, Z., Gao, Q., Tang, N., Shui, R., & Shen, M.
(2015) Organization principles in visual working memory: Evidence from sequential stimulus display. Cognition, 146, 277–288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garavan, H.
(1998) Serial attention within working memory. Memory & Cognition, 26, 263–276. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
García Mayo M. D. P., & González Alonso, J.
(2015) L3 acquisition: A focus on cognitive approaches. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 127–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gardiner, J. M., Ramponi, C., & Richardson-Klavehn, A.
(1998) Experiences of remembering, knowing, and guessing. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 1–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gardner, H.
(1993) Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences (2nd ed.). London: Fontana.Google Scholar
(1999) Intelligence reframed: Multiple intelligences for the 21st century. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. C., & Lambert, W. E.
(1972) Attitudes and motivation in second language learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Gardner-Chloros, P.
(2009) Code-switching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gass, S., & Lee, J.
(2011) Working memory capacity, inhibitory control, and proficiency in a second language. In M. S. Schmid & W. Lowie (Eds.), Modeling bilingualism: From structure to chaos: In honor of Kees de Bot (pp. 59–84). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gass, S., & Mackey, A.
(2002) Frequency effects and second language acquisition. A complex picture? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 249–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E.
(2006) Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513–543. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S.
(1992) Nature’s mind: The biological roots of thinking, emotions, sexuality, language, and intelligence. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
(1998) The mind’s past. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
(2011) Who’s in charge? Free will and the science of the brain. New York, NY: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C.
(2004) Evolution and cognitive development. In R. Burgess & K. MacDonald (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on human development (pp. 99–133). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Gierhan, S. M. E.
(2013a) Brain networks for language: Anatomy and functional roles of neural pathways supporting language comprehension and repetition. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences.Google Scholar
(2013b) Connections for auditory language in the human brain. Brain & Language, 127, 205–221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glimcher, P. W., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Poldrack, R. A.
(Eds.) (2009) Neuroeconomics: Decision making and the brain. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goddard, C., & Wierzbicka, A.
(Eds.) (1994) Semantic and lexical universals: Theory and empirical findings. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A.
(1995) Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, E.
(2009) The new executive brain: Frontal lobes in a complex world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldrick, M., Putnam, M., & Schwarz, L.
(2016a) Coactivation in bilingual grammars: A computational account of code mixing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 857–876. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016b) The future of code mixing research: Integrating psycholinguistic and formal grammatical theories. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 903–906. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, S., Naglieri, J. A., Princiotta, D., & Otero, T. M.
(2014) Introduction: A history of executive functioning as a theoretical and clinical construct. In S. Goldstein & J. A. Naglieri (Eds.), Handbook of executive functioning (pp. 3–12). New York, NY: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., & Ferreira, V. S.
(2009) Should I stay or should I switch? A cost–benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 640–665. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M.
(1993) Goal achievement: The role of intentions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 141–185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1999) Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P., & Brandstätter, V.
(1997) Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 73, 186–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodfriend, G.
(1986) Variation in land-snail shell form and size and its causes: A review. Systematic Biology, 35, 204–223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gooding, D. C., & Tallent, K. A.
(2003) Spatial, object, and affective working memory in social anhedonia: An exploratory study. Schizophrenia Research, 63, 247–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gray, J., Schaefer, A., Braver, T. S., & Most, S. B.
(2005) Affect and the resolution of cognitive control dilemmas. In L. F. Barrett, P. M. Niedenthal, & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Emotion and consciousness (pp. 51–66). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Green, D. W.
(1986) Control, activation and resource. Brain and Language, 27, 210–223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J.
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 515–530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: Neural adaptation and reserve. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 1–10.Google Scholar
Green, D. W., & Wei, L.
(2014) A control process model of code-switching. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 499–511. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Code-switching and language control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 883–884. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, A. G.
(1980) The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35, 603–618. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P.
(1967) Logic and conversation. Reprinted in H. P. Grice (1989) Studies in the way of words (pp. 1–143). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F.
(1982) Life with two languages: An introduction to bilingualism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(2001) The bilingual’s language modes. In J. L. Nicol (Ed.), One mind, two languages: Bilingual language processing (pp. 1–22). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
(2010) Bilingual: Life and reality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) The Complementarity Principle and its impact on processing, acquisition, and dominance. In C. Silva-Corvalán & J. Treffers-Daller (Eds.), Language dominance in bilinguals: Issues of measurement and operationalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F., & Li, P.
(2013) The psycholinguistics of bilingualism. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J.
(1999) Emotion regulation: Past, present, future. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 551–573. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grundy, J. G., & Timmer, K.
(2017) Bilingualism and working memory capacity: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Second Language Research, 33, 325–340. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guo, T., Liu, H., Misra, M., & Kroll, J. F.
(2011) Local and global inhibition in bilingual word production: fMRI evidence from Chinese–English bilinguals. NeuroImage, 56, 2300–2309. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gürel, A.
(2004) Selectivity in L2-induced L1 attrition: A psycholinguistic account. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 53–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haegens, S., Osipova, D., Oostenveld, R., & Jensen, O.
(2010) Somatosensory working memory performance in humans depends on both engagement and disengagement of regions in a distributed network. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 26–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hagoort, P., & van Berkum, J.
(2007) Beyond the sentence given. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 801–811. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J.
(2012) The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K., & Hasan R.
(1976) Cohesion in English. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Hammarberg, B.
(2001) Roles of L1 and L2 in L3 production and acquisition. In J. Cenoz, B. Hufeisen, & U. Jessner (Eds.), Cross-linguistic influence in third language acquisition: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 21–41). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Handjaras, G., Ricciardi, E., Leo, A., Lenci, A., Cecchetti, L., Cosottini, M.
, et al. (2016) How concepts are encoded in the human brain: A modality independent, category-based cortical organization of semantic knowledge. Neuroimage, 135, 232–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heller, M.
(1988) Codeswitching: Anthropological and sociolinguistic perspectives. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hemmi, C.
(2014) Dual identities perceived by bilinguals. In S. Mercer & M. Williams (Eds.), Multiple perspectives on the self in SLA (pp. 75–91). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hepler, J., & Albarracín, D.
(2013) Complete unconscious control: Using (in)action primes to demonstrate completely unconscious activation of inhibitory control mechanisms. Cognition, 128, 271–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., Clausenius-Kalman, H. L., Ronderos, J., Castilla-Earls, A. P., Sun, L., Weiss, S. D., & Young, D. R.
(2019) Neuroemergentism: A framework for studying cognition and the brain. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 49, 214–223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Herrmann, C. S., Munk, M. H. J., & Engel, A. K.
(2004) Cognitive functions of gamma-band activity: Memory match and utilization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 347–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hervais-Adelman, A. G., Moser-Mercer, B., & Golestani, N.
(2011) Executive control of language in the bilingual brain: Integrating the evidence from neuroimaging to neuropsychology. Frontiers in Psychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hikosaka, O., Kim, H. F., Yasuda, M., & Yamamoto, S.
(2014) Basal ganglia circuits for reward value-guided behavior. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 37, 289–306. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hilgard, E. R.
(1986) Divided consciousness: Multiple controls in human thought and action, expanded edition. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Hilles, S.
(1986) Interlanguage and the pro-drop parameter. Second Language Research, 2, 33–52.Google Scholar
Hirschfeld, L. A., & Gelman, S. A.
(Eds.) (1994) Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hood, B.
(2012) The self illusion: How the social brain creates identity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hoshino, N., & Thierry, G.
(2014) Language selection during speech production in bilingual speakers. In E. M. Thomas & I. Mennen (Eds.), Advances in the study of bilingualism. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hummel, K. M.
(2009) Aptitude, phonological memory, and second language proficiency in nonnovice adult learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 225–249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Inzlicht, M., Shenhav, A., & Olivola, C. Y.
(2018) The effort paradox: Effort is both costly and valued. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 337–349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ivaz, L., Costa, A., & Duñabeitia, J. A.
(2016) The emotional impact of being myself: Emotions and foreign-language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 489–496. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E.
(2009) Emotion theory and research: Highlights, unanswered questions, and emerging issues. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R.
(1987) Consciousness and the computational mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1990) Semantic structures. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1996) Conceptual semantics and cognitive linguistics. Cognitive Linguistics, 7, 93–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997) The architecture of the language faculty. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2002) Foundations of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R., & Lehrdahl, F.
(2006) The capacity for music: What is it, and what’s special about it? Cognition, 100, 33–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jackson, R., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Pobric, G.
(2015) The timing of anterior temporal lobe involvement in semantic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 1388–1396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C.
(2001) Applying the working memory model to the study of atypical development. In J. Andrade (Ed.), Working memory in perspective (pp. 126–150). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Jarvis, S., & Pavlenko, A.
(2008) Crosslinguistic influence in language and cognition. New York, NY: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jefferies, E.
(2013) The neural basis of semantic cognition: Converging evidence from neuropsychology, neuroimaging and TMS. Cortex, 49, 611–625. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N.
(2015) Six decades of research on lexical representation and processing in bilinguals. In J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing (pp. 29–84). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnston, E., & Olson, L.
(2015) The feeling brain: The biology and psychology of emotions. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Johnston, W. A., & Dark, V. J.
(1986) Selective attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 37, 43–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jokisch, D., & Jensen, O.
(2007) Modulation of gamma and alpha activity during a working memory task engaging the dorsal or ventral stream. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 3244–3251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A.
(2015) Working memory and sentence processing: A commentary. In Z. Wen, M. B. Mota, & A. McNeill (Eds.), Working memory in second language acquisition and processing (pp. 125–135). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. W.
(2011) Aspects of working memory in L2 learning. Language Teaching, 44, 137–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jylkkä, J., Lehtonen, J., Lindholm, F., Kuusakoski, A., & Laine, M.
(2017) The relationship between general executive functions and bilingual switching and monitoring in language production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1–18. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D.
(2011) Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Penguin.Google Scholar
Karalis, N., Dejean, C., Chaudun, F., Khoder, S., Rozeske, R. R., Wurtz, H., Bagur, S., Benchenane, K., Sirota, A., Courtin, J., & Herry, C.
(2016) 4-Hz oscillations synchronize prefrontal–amygdala circuits during fear behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 19, 605–616. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A.
(1992) Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kaschub, C. E.
(2008) Emotional working memory: An individual differences approach to understanding attention control (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest. (UMI 3416451).Google Scholar
Kasper, G., & Schmidt, R.
(1996) Developmental issues in interlanguage pragmatics. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 149–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Katus, T., & Andersen, S. K.
(2016) The role of spatial attention in tactile short-term memory. In P. Jolicoeur, C. Lefebvre, & J. Martinez-Trujillo (Eds.), Mechanisms of sensory working memory: Attention and performance XXV (pp. 266–282). London: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Katzman, R.
(1993) Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 43,1, 13–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kellerman, E.
(1979) Transfer and non-transfer: Where we are now. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 2, 37–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1986) An eye for an eye: Crosslinguistic constraints on the development of the L2 lexicon. In E. Kellerman & M. Sharwood Smith (Eds.), Crosslinguistic influence in second language acquisition (pp. 35–48). New York, NY: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Kiefer, M., & Barsalou, L. W.
(2013) Grounding the human conceptual system in perception, action, and internal states. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline (pp. 381–407). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kihlstrom, J. F.
(1987) The cognitive unconscious. Science 237, 1445–1452. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997) Consciousness and me-ness. In J. D. Cohen & J. W. Schooler (Eds.), Scientific approaches to consciousness (pp. 451–468). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Kihlstrom, J. F., & Klein, S. B.
(2006) Self-knowledge and self-awareness. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 818, 4–17.Google Scholar
Kim, S. H. O., & Starks, D.
(2008) The role of emotions in L1 attrition: The case of Korean-English late bilinguals in New Zealand. International Journal of Bilingualism, 12, 303–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kirk, N., Kempe, V., Scott-Brown, K. C., Philipp, A., & Declerck, M.
(2018) Can monolinguals be like bilinguals? Evidence from dialect switching. Cognition, 170,164–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kitzbichler, M. G., Henson, R. N. A., Smith, M. L., Nathan, P. J., & Bullmore, E. T.
(2011) Cognitive effort drives workspace configuration of human brain functional networks. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 8259–8270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kleinman, D., & Gollan, T. H.
(2016) Speaking two languages for the price of one: Bypassing language control mechanisms via accessibility-driven switches. Psychological Science, 27, 700–714. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kluft, R. P.
(1996) Dissociative identity disorder. In L. K. Michelson & W. J. Ray (Eds.), Handbook of dissociation: Theoretical, empirical, and clinical perspectives (pp. 337–366). New York, NY: Plenum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N.
(2007) Attention and consciousness: Two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 16–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Attention and consciousness: Related but different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 103–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Köpke, B., & Nespoulous, J.-L.
(2006) Working memory performance in expert and novice interpreters. Interpreting, 8, 1–23. <hal-00981122> DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Köpke, B., & Signorelli, T.
(2012) Methodological aspects of working memory assessment in simultaneous interpreters. International Journal of Bilingualism, 16, 183–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krashen, S.
(1976) Formal and informal linguistic environments in language learning and language acquisition. TESOL Quarterly, 10, 157–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krashen, S. D.
(1985) The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Krashen, S.
(1989) We acquire vocabulary and spelling by reading: Additional evidence for the input hypothesis. Modern Language Journal, 73, 440–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauzlis, R. J., Bollimunta, A., Arcizet, F., & Wang, L.
(2014) Attention as an effect not a cause. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 457–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., & Wodniecka, Z.
(2006) Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: Arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 119–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Christoffels, I., & Bajo, T.
(Eds.) (2013) Bilingualism and cognitive control. Frontiers in Psychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & de Groot, A. M. B.
(Eds.) (2005) Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 433–453). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bice, K., & Perrotti, L.
(2015) Bilingualism, mind, and brain. Annual Review of Linguistics, 1, 377–394. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Gullifer, J. W., McClain, R., Rossi, E., & Martín, M. C.
(2015) Selection and control in bilingual comprehension and production. In J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing (pp. 485–507). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E.
(1994) Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory & Language, 33, 149–174. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., van Hell, J. G., Tokowicz, N., & Green, D. W.
(2010) The Revised Hierarchical Model: A critical review and assessment. Bilingualism (Cambridge, England), 13(3), 373–381. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J.
(1986) Motivation and information processing: A new look at decision making, dynamic change, and action control. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 404–434). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kuroda, R.
(2014) How a single gene twists a snail. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 54, 677–687. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kurzban, R.
(2010) Why everyone (else) is a hypocrite: Evolution and the modular mind. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
(2016) The sense of effort. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 67–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G.
(1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Sage, K., Jones, R. W., & Mayberry, E. J.
(2010) Coherent concepts are computed in the anterior temporal lobes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 107, 2717–2722. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lamme, V. A. F.
(2003) Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 12–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lane, R. D., & Nadel, L.
(2000) Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R.
(1987, 1991) Foundations of cognitive grammar. 2 vols. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D.
(2008) Feature-assembly in second language acquisition. In J. Liceras, H. Zobl, & H. Goodluck (Eds.), The role of features in second language acquisition (pp. 106–140). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
LaRocque, J. J., Lewis-Peacock, J. A., Drysdale, A. T., Oberauer, K., & Postle, B. R.
(2013) Decoding attended information in short-term memory: An EEG study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 127–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lau, H. C., & Passingham, R. E.
(2007) Unconscious activation of the cognitive control system in the human prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 5805–5811. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lazarus, R. S.
(1991) Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 819–834. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leary, M. R.
(2007) Motivational and emotional aspects of the self. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 317–344. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
LeDoux, J.
(1996) The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
(2012) Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron, 73, 653–676. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Anxious: The modern mind in the age of anxiety. London: Oneworld.Google Scholar
Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. S.
(1983) A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lehto, J.
(1996) Are executive function tests dependent on working memory capacity? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 29–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M.
(1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levy, B. J., & Anderson, M. C.
(2002) Inhibitory processes and the control of memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 299–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewis-Peacock, J. A., Drysdale, A. T., Oberauer, K., & Postle, B. R.
(2012) Neural evidence for a distinction between short-term memory and the focus of attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 61–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, P., & Farkas, I.
(2002) A self-organizing connectionist model of bilingual processing. In R. Heredia & J. Altarriba (Eds.), Bilingual sentence processing (pp. 59–85). Amsterdam: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, D.
(1982) The language lottery: Toward a biology of grammars. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Linck, J. A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J. T., & Bunting, M. F.
(2014) Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 861–883. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liu, H., Liang, L., Zhang, L., Lu, Y., & Chen, B.
(2017) Modulatory role of inhibition during language switching: Evidence from evoked and induced oscillatory activity. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21, 57–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Logan, G. D., van Zandt, T., Verbruggen, F., & Wagenmakers, E.-J.
(2014) On the ability to inhibit thought and action: General and special theories of an act of control. Psychological Review, 121, 66–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luciana, M., Burgund, E. D., Berman, M., & Hanson, K. L.
(2001) Effects of tryptophan loading on verbal, spatial and affective working memory functions in healthy adults. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 15, 219–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Grady, C. L.
(2011) Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 16808–16813. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lundqvist, D., & Öhman, A.
(2005) Caught by the evil eye: Nonconscious information processing, emotion, and attention to facial stimuli. In L. F. Barrett, P. M. Niedenthal, & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Emotion and consciousness (pp. 97–122). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mack, A., & Rock, I.
(1998) Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacSwan, J.
(1999) A minimalist approach to intrasentential code-switching. New York, NY: Garland.Google Scholar
(2000) The architecture of the bilingual language faculty: Evidence from intrasentential code switching. Bilingualism, Language and Cognition 3, 37–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A.
(2008) A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology–Paris, 102, 59–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahootian, S., & Santorini, B.
(1996) Code switching and the complement/adjunct distinction. Linguistic Inquiry, 27, 464–479.Google Scholar
Mameli, M.
(2004) The role of emotions in ecological and practical rationality. In D. Evans & P. Cruse (Eds.), Emotion, evolution, and rationality (pp. 159–178). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mandler, G.
(1975) Consciousness: Respectable, useful, and probably necessary. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium (pp. 229–254). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Marien, H., Custers, R., Hassin, R. R., & Aarts, H.
(2012) Unconscious goal activation and the hijacking of the executive function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 399–415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Markus, H., & Nurius, P.
(1986) Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954–969. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Markus, H., & Wurf, E.
(1987) The dynamic self-concept: A social psychological perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 299–337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, R., & Barresi, J.
(2012) The rise and fall of soul and self: An intellectual history of personal identity. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., & Romani, C.
(1994) Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension: A multiple-components view. Neuropsychology, 8, 506–523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. C., Shelton, J. R., & Yaffee, L. S.
(1994) Language processing and working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 83–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martínez-Manrique, F.
(2010) On the distinction between semantic and conceptual representation. Dialectica, 64, 57–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marton, K.
(2015) Theoretically driven experiments may clarify questions about the bilingual advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 37–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marton, K., Goral, M., Campanelli, L., Yoon, J., & Obler, L. K.
(2017) Executive control mechanisms in bilingualism: Beyond speed of processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 613–631. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Masoura, E. V., & Gathercole, S. E.
(2005) Contrasting contributions of phonological short-term memory and long-term knowledge to vocabulary learning in a foreign language. Memory, 13, 422–429. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
May, J.
(2001) Specifying the central executive may require complexity. In J. Andrade (Ed.), Working memory in perspective (pp. 261–277). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
McClelland, J.
(1981) Retrieving general and specific information from stored knowledge of specifics. Proceedings of the Third Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 170–172.Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L., & Rogers, T. T.
(2003) The parallel distributed processing approach to semantic cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 310–322. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E.
(1981) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McCulloch, W. S.
(1945) A heterarchy of values determined by the topology of nervous nets. Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics, 7, 89–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1988) Embodiments of mind. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
McElree, B.
(2006) Accessing recent events. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 46, 155–200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McGovern, K., & Baars, J.
(2007) Cognitive theories of consciousness. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 177–205). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mercer, S., & Williams, M.
(Eds.) (2014) Multiple perspectives on the self in SLA. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meteyard, L., Cuadrado, S. R., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G.
(2012) Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex, 48, 788–804. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meuter, R.
(2009) Neurolinguistic contributions to understanding the bilingual mental lexicon. In A. Pavlenko (Ed.), The bilingual mental lexicon: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 1–25). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meuter, R., & Allport, A.
(1999) Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 25–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E.
(1997) A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review, 104, 3–65. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mikels, J. A., Larkin, G. R., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Carstensen, L. L.
(2005) Divergent trajectories in the aging mind: Changes in working memory for affective versus visual information with age. Psychology and Aging, 20, 542–553. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mikels, J. A., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Beyer, J. A., & Fredrickson, B. L.
(2008) Emotion and working memory: Evidence for domain-specific processes for affective maintenance. Emotion, 8, 256–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, E. K., & Buschman, T. J.
(2014) Neural mechanisms for the executive control of attention. In S. Kastner & A. C. Nobre (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A.
(1995) WordNet: A lexical database for English. Communications of the ACM, 38(11), 39–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H.
(1960) Plans and the structure of behavior. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Milroy, L., & Muysken, P.
(1995) One speaker, two languages: Cross-disciplinary perspectives on code-switching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Minsky, M.
(1974) A framework for representing knowledge. Reprinted in P. Winston (Ed.), The psychology of computer vision (pp. 211–277). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Misra, M., Guo, T., Bobb, S. C., & Kroll, J. F.
(2012) When bilinguals choose a single word to speak: Electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 224–237. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A.
(2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., & Shah, P.
(Eds.) (1999) Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mongillo, G., Barak, O., & Tsodyks, M.
(2008) Synaptic theory of working memory. Science, 319, 1543–1546. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montague, R.
(2006) Your brain is (almost) perfect: How we make decisions. New York, NY: Plume.Google Scholar
Moore, S. C., & Oaksford, M.
(Eds.) (2002) Emotional cognition: From brain to behaviour. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moray, N.
(1969) Attention: Selective processes in vision and hearing. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Morillas-Romero, A., Tortella-Feliu, M., Balle, M., & Bornas, X.
(2014) Spontaneous emotion regulation and attentional control. Emotion, 15, 162–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morris, J. S., DeGelder, B., Weiskrantz, L., & Dolan, R. J.
(2001) Differential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. Brain, 124, 1241–1252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morrison, S. E., & Salzman, C. D.
(2010) Re-valuing the amygdala. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20, 221–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Müller, N.
(1998) UG access without parameter setting: A longitudinal study of (L1 Italian) German as a second language. In M.-L. Beck (Ed.), Morphology and its interfaces in L2 knowledge (pp. 115–163). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Munakata, Y., Herd, S. A., Chatham, C. H., Depue, B. E., Banich, M. T., & O’Reilly, R. C.
(2011) A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 453–459. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F.
(2000) Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Myers-Scotton, C.
(1993) Dueling languages: Grammatical structure in codeswitching. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2006) Multiple voices: An introduction to bilingualism. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Naccache, L., Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Habert, M.-O., Guichart-Gomez, E., Galanaude, D., & Willer, J.-C.
(2005) Effortless control: Executive attention and conscious feeling of mental effort are dissociable. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1318–1328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U.
(1997) The self in culture. In U. Neisser & D. A. Jopling (Eds.), The conceptual self in context: Culture, experience, self-understanding (pp. 3–12). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nobre, A. C., & Mesulam, M-M.
(2014) Large-scale networks for attentional biases. In S. Kastner & A. C. Nobre (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T.
(1986) Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation (pp. 1–18). New York, NY: Plenum Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., & Panksepp, J.
(2006) Self-referential processing in our brain – A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage, 31, 440–457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norton, B.
(2013) Identity and language learning: Extending the conversation. 2nd ed. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oberauer, K.
(2009) Design for a working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 51, 45–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oberauer, K., Farrell, S., Jarrold, C., & Lewandowsky, S.
(2016) What limits working memory capacity? Psychological Bulletin, 142, 758–799. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oberauer, K., & Hein, L.
(2012) Attention to information in working memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 164–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., Silvers, J. A., & Buhle, J. T.
(2012) Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: A synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251, E1–E24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Grady, W.
(2005) Syntactic carpentry: An emergentist approach to syntax. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Öhman, A.
(2006) Making sense of emotion: Evolution, reason & the brain. Daedalus, 135(3), 33–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Onnis, L., & Spivey, M.
(2012) Toward a new scientific visualization for the language sciences. Information, 3, 124–150. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A.
(1988) The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K., & Greenberg, I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O.
(2015) Should the search for bilingual advantages in executive functioning continue? Cortex PMID: 26586100Google Scholar
Palva, J. M., Monto, S., Kulashekhar, S., & Palva, S.
(2010) Neuronal synchrony reveals working memory networks and predicts individual memory capacity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 107, 7580–7585. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J.
(1998) The periconscious substrates of consciousness: Affective states and the evolutionary origins of the SELF. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5, 566–582.Google Scholar
(2005) Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Consciousness & Cognition, 14, 30–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Neurologizing the psychology of affects: How appraisal-based constructivism and basic emotion theory can coexist. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 281–296.
https://doi.org/
. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J., & Biven, L.
(2012) The archaeology of mind: Neuroevolutionary origins of human emotion. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Paradis, M.
(1993) Linguistic, psycholinguistic, and neurolinguistic aspects of “interference” in bilingual speakers: The activation threshold hypothesis. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 9, 133–145.Google Scholar
Parasuraman, R.
(1998) The attentive brain: Issues and prospects. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.), The attentive brain (pp. 3–15). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Parodi, T., & Tsimpli, I.
(2005) ‘Real’ and apparent optionality in second language grammars: Finiteness and pronouns in null operator structures. Second Language Research, 21, 250–285. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parvizi, J., & Damasio, A.
(2001) Consciousness and the brainstem. Cognition, 79, 135–159. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pashler, H.
(1995) Attention and visual perception: Analyzing divided attention. In S. M. Kosslyn & D. N. Osherson (Eds.), An invitation to cognitive science (2nd ed): Vol. 2. Visual cognition (pp. 71–100). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pavlenko, A.
(1999) New approaches to concepts in bilingual memory. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 209–230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000) New approaches to concepts in bilingual memory. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3, 1–4. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005a) Bilingualism and thought. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 433–453). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2005b) Emotions and multilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2009a) Conceptual representation in the bilingual lexicon and second language vocabulary learning. In A. Pavlenko (Ed.), The bilingual mental lexicon: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 125–160). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009b) Time for new metaphors? In A. Pavlenko (Ed.), The bilingual mental lexicon: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. xi–xv). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(Ed.) (2009c) The bilingual mental lexicon: Interdisciplinary approaches. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B., Fernandez, S., & Oller, D.
(1993) Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43, 93–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peck, C. J., & Salzman, C. D.
(2014) Amygdala neural activity reflects spatial attention towards stimuli promising reward or threatening punishment. eLife, 3:e04478. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pegna, A. J., Landis, T., & Khateb, A.
(2008) Electrophysiological evidence for early non-conscious processing of fearful facial expressions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 70, 127–36. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Perani, D., & Abutalebi, J.
(2015) Bilingualism, dementia, cognitive and neural reserve. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 618–625. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Perani, D., Farsad, M, Ballarini, T., Lubian, F., Malpetti, M, Fracchetti, A., Magnani, G., March, A., & Abutalebi, J.
(2017) The impact of bilingualism on brain reserve and metabolic connectivity in Alzheimer’s dementia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 114, 1690–1695. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Pessoa, L.
(2013) The cognitive-emotional brain: From interactions to integration. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Attention, motivation, and emotion. In S. Kastner & A. C. Nobre (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of attention (pp. 725–752). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfaff, C.
(1979) Constraints on language-mixing: Intrasentential code-switching and borrowing in Spanish/English. Language, 55, 291–318. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Phelps, E. A.
(2006) Emotion and cognition: Insights from studies of the human amygdala. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 27–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pietrini, P., Furey, M. L., Ricciardi, E., Gobbini, M. I., Wu, W.-H. C., Cohen, L., Guazzelli, M., & Haxby, J. V.
(2004) Beyond sensory images: Object-based representation in the human ventral pathway. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 101, 5658–5663. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1994) The language instinct: The new science of language and mind. London: Penguin. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997) How the mind works. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
(2007) The stuff of thought. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Platel, H., Baron, J.-C., Desgranges, B., Bernard, F., & Eustache, F.
(2003) Semantic and episodic memory of music are subserved by distinct neural networks. NeuroImage, 20, 244–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pliatsikas, C., & Luk, G.
(2016) Executive control in bilinguals: A concise review on fMRI studies. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 699–705. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pobric, G., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A.
(2010) Amodal semantic representations depend on both anterior temporal lobes: Evidence from repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuropsychologia, 48, 1336–1342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poplack, S.
(1980) Sometimes I’ll start a sentence in English y termino en español: Toward a typology of code-switching. Linguistics, 18, 581–618. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1981) Syntactic structure and social function of codeswitching. In R. P. Durán (Ed.), Latino language and communicative behavior (pp. 169–184). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I.
(1994) Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 91, 7398–7403. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Attention in a social world. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Postle, B. R.
(2015) The cognitive neuroscience of visual short-term memory. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 1, 40–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Neural bases of the short-term retention of visual information. In P. Jolicoeur, C. Lefebvre, & J. Martinez-Trujillo (Eds.), Mechanisms of sensory working memory: Attention and performance XXV (pp. 43–58). London: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L.
(2000) Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second Language Research, 16, 103–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prinz, J. J.
(2012) The conscious brain: How attention engenders experience. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pritchett, D. L., Siegle, J. H., Deister, C. A., & Moore, C. I.
(2015) For things needing your attention: The role of neocortical gamma in sensory perception. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 31, 254–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F.
(1999) Words in the brain’s language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 253–336. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ranganath, C., & Blumenfeld, R. S.
(2005) Doubts about double dissociations between short- and long-term memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 374–380. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raynor, J. O., & McFarlin, D. B.
(1986) Motivation and the self-system. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 315–349). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rieber, R. W.
(2006) The bifurcation of the self: The history and theory of dissociation and its disorders. New York: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robertson, D.
(2000) Variability in the use of the English article system by Chinese learners of English. Second Language Research, 16, 135–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robertson, D., & Sorace, A.
(1999) Losing the V2 constraint. In E. C. Klein & G. Martohardjono (Eds.), The development of second language grammars: A generative approach, (pp. 317–361). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S.
(1995) Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Romani, C., & Martin, R.
(1999) A deficit in the short-term retention of lexical–semantic information: Forgetting words but remembering a story. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 56–77. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ross, M.
(1989) The relation of implicit theories to the construction of personal histories. Psychological Review, 96, 341–357. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J.
(2011) L3 syntactic transfer selectivity and typological determinacy: The Typological Primacy Model. Second Language Research, 27, 107–127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Linguistic and cognitive motivations for the Typological Primacy Model (TPM) of third language (L3) transfer: Timing of acquisition and proficiency considered. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 179–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., Iverson, M., & Judy, T.
(2011) Introduction: Some notes on the generative study of L3 acquisition. Second Language Research, 27, 5–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roux, F., & Uhlhaas, P. J.
(2014) Working memory and neural oscillations: Alpha–gamma versus theta–gamma codes for distinct WM information? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 16–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roy, M., Shohamy, D., & Wager, T. D.
(2012) Ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical systems and the generation of affective meaning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 147–156. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ruchkin, D. S., Grafman, J., Cameron, K., & Berndt, R. S.
(2003) Working memory retention systems: A state of activated long-term memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 709–777. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., & Ortony, A.
(1977) The representation of knowledge in memory. In R. C. Anderson, R. J. Spiro, & W. E. Montague (Eds.), Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge (pp. 99–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L.
(1982) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and expansions of the model. Psychological Review, 89, 60–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salinas, E., & Sejnowski, T. J.
(2001) Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 539–550. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sander, D., Grafman, J., & Zalla, T.
(2003) The human amygdala: An evolved system for relevance detection. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 14, 303–316. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sandler, W.
(2012) The phonological organization of sign languages. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6, 162–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, D., & Poplack, S.
(1981) A formal grammar for code-switching. Papers in Linguistics, 14, 3–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sapir, E.
(1949) Selected writings of Edward Sapir. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Schaeffner, S., Fibla, L., & Philipp, A.
(2016) Bimodal language switching: New insights from signing and typing. Journal of Memory and Language, 94, 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P.
(1977) Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., Shorr, A., & Johnstone, T.
(Eds.) (2001) Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. W.
(1990) The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, 129–158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R.
(1993) Awareness and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 13, 206–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995) Consciousness and foreign language learning: A tutorial on the role of attention and awareness in learning. In R. Schmidt (Ed.), Attention and awareness in foreign language learning (pp. 3–63). Honolulu, HI: Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center, University of Hawai‘i.Google Scholar
(2001) Attention. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 3–32). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R., & Frota, S. N.
(1986) Developing basic conversational ability in a second language: A case study of an adult learner of Portuguese. In R. R. Day (Ed.), Talking to learn: Conversation in second language acquisition (pp. 237–326). Rowley, MA: Newbury.Google Scholar
Schmitt, E.
(2002) No more reductions! To the problem of evaluation of language attrition data. In M. Schmid, B. Köpke, M. Keijzer, & L. Weilemar (Eds.), First language attrition: Interdisciplinary perspectives on methodological issues (pp. 299–315). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., & Pimm-Smith, M.
(1997) Consciousness as a message aware control mechanism to modulate cognitive processing. In J. D. Cohen & J. W. Schooler (Eds.), Scientific approaches to consciousness (pp. 65–80). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M.
(1977) Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review 84, 1–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schumann, J. H.
(1997) The neurobiology of affect in language. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Schwartz, A.
(2015) Bilingual lexical access during written sentence comprehension. In J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing (pp. 327–348). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwieter, J. W.
(Ed.) (2015) The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(Ed.) (2016) Cognitive control and consequences of multilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seager, W.
(2002) Emotional introspection. Consciousness and Cognition, 11, 666–687. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sergerie, K., Chochol, C., & Armony, J. L.
(2008) The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 811–830. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Service, E., Simola, M., Metsänheimo, O., & Maury, S.
(2002) Bilingual working memory span is affected by language skill. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 383–408. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sestieri, C., Shulman, G. L., & Corbetta, M.
(2012) Orienting to the environment: Separate contributions of dorsal and ventral frontoparietal attention networks. In G. R. Mangun (Ed.), The neuroscience of attention: Attentional control and selection. Oxford Scholarship Online. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M.
(1981) Consciousness-raising and the second language learner. Applied Linguistics, 2, 159–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1986) Comprehension versus acquisition: Two ways of processing input. Applied Linguistics, 7, 239–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M. A.
(1993) Input enhancement in instructed SLA: Theoretical bases. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 15, 165–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M.
(1996) The Garden of Eden and beyond: On second language processing. CLCS Occasional Paper no. 44, Trinity College, Dublin.Google Scholar
(2004) In two minds about grammar: On the interaction of linguistic and metalinguistic knowledge in performance. Transactions of the Philological Society, 102, 255–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014a) Can you learn to love grammar and so make it grow? On the role of affect in L2. Essential topics in applied linguistics and multilingualism: Studies in honor of David Singleton (pp. 3–20). Berlin: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M. A.
(2014b) In search of conceptual frameworks for relating brain activity to language function. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 716. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M.
(2017a) Active-passive bilingualism and functional distance between L1 and L2 explained within a unified cognitive perspective. In A. Ardila, A. Cieślicka, R. Heredia, & R. Rosselli (Eds.), Psychology of bilingualism: The cognitive world of bilinguals (pp. 157–158). Berlin: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017b) Introducing language and cognition: A map of the mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017c) Language and affective processing implemented within a crossdisciplinary conceptual framework. Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 53, 43–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017d) Working with working memory and language. Second Language Research, 33, 291–298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019a) The compatibility within a modular framework of emergent and dynamical processes in mind and brain. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 49, 240–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019b) Representing representation: Three perspectives and the case of grammatical gender. In R. Slabakova, J. Corbet, L. Dominguez, A. Dudley, & A. Wellington (Eds.), Explorations in second language processing and representation (pp. 2–40). Acquisition Conference. Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M., & Truscott, J.
(2006) Full Transfer Full Access: A processing-oriented interpretation. In S. Unsworth, T. Parodi, A. Sorace, & M. Young-Scholten (Eds.), Paths of development in L1 and L2 acquisition (pp. 201–216). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Consciousness and language: A processing perspective. In E. K. Perry, D. Collerton, F. E. N. LeBeau, & H. Ashton (Eds.), New horizons in the neuroscience of consciousness (pp. 129–138). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014a) Explaining input enhancement: A MOGUL perspective. IRAL, 52, 253–281.Google Scholar
(2014b) The multilingual mind: A modular processing perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M., Truscott, J., & Hawkins, R.
(2013) Explaining change in transition grammars. In J. Herschensohn & M. Young-Scholten (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 560–580). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shen, M., Xu, H., Zhang, H., Shui, R., Zhang, M., & Zhou, J.
(2015) The working memory Ponzo illusion: Involuntary integration of visuospatial information stored in visual working memory. Cognition, 14, 26–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shenhav, A., Musslick, S., Lieder, F., Kool, W., Griffiths, T. L., Cohen, J. D., & Botvinick, M. M.
(2017) Toward a rational and mechanistic account of mental effort. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 40, 99–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W.
(1977) Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shivde, G., & Anderson, M. C.
(2011) On the existence of semantic working memory: Evidence for direct semantic maintenance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1342–1370. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sholl, A., Sankaranarayanan, A., & Kroll, J.
(1995) Transfer between picture naming and translation: A test of asymmetries in bilingual memory. Psychological Science, 6, 45–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shook, A., & Marian, V.
(2013) The Bilingual Language Interaction Network for Comprehension of Speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 304–324. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silvers, J., Buhle, J. T., & Ochsner, K. N.
(2013) The neuroscience of emotion regulation: Basic mechanisms and their role in development, aging, and psychopathology. In K. N. Ochsner & S. Kosslyn (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive neuroscience: Volume 2: The cutting edges. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C.
(1985) Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E.
(2003) Development as a dynamic system. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 343–348. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2000) Syntactic optionality in non-native grammars. Second Language Research, 16, 93–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D.
(2005) Modularity and relevance: How can a massively modular mind be flexible and context-sensitive? In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), The innate mind: Structure and contents (pp. 53–68). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D.
(1986) Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
(1995) Postface to the second edition of Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
(2002) Pragmatics, modularity and mind‐reading. Mind and Language, 17, 3–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stern, Y.
(2009) Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2015–2028. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L.
(2007) On the interdependence of cognition and emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 1212–1237. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Storms, G., Ameel, E., & Malt, B. C.
(2015) Development of cross-language lexical influence. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 18, 529–547. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stuphorn, V.
(2015) Neural mechanisms of response inhibition. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 1, 64–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suhler, C. L., & Churchland, P. S.
(2009) Control: Conscious and otherwise. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 341–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Szmalec, A., Brysbaert, M., & Duyck, W.
(2013) Working memory and (second) language processing. In J. Altarriba & L. Isurin (Eds.), Memory, language, and bilingualism: Theoretical and applied approaches (pp. 74–94). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tamietto, M., Castelli, L., Vighetti, S., Perozzo, P., Geminiani, G., Weiskrantz, L., & de Gelder, B.
(2009) Unseen facial and bodily expressions trigger fast emotional reactions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 106, 17661–17666. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taylor, F.
(2013) Self and identity in adolescent foreign language learning. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taylor, K. I., Devereux, B. J., & Tyler, L. K.
(2011) Conceptual structure: Towards an integrated neurocognitive account. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 1368–1401. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Terrell, T. D.
(1991) The role of grammar instruction in a communicative approach. Modern Language Journal, 75, 52–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tettamanti, M., Rognoni, E., Cafiero, R., Costa, T., Galati, D., & Perani, D.
(2012) Distinct pathways of neural coupling for different basic emotions. NeuroImage, 59, 1804–1817. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thagard, P.
(2006) Hot thought: Mechanisms and applications of emotional cognition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M., & van Heuven, W.
(2005) Computational models of bilingual comprehension. In J. Kroll & A. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 202–225). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Thornton, J.
(2003) Life-span learning: A developmental perspective. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 57, 55–76. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C.
(1932) Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2003) Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L.
(1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Toribio, A. J.
(2001) On the emergence of bilingual codeswitching competence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 203–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Towse, J. N., & Houston-Price, C. M. T.
(2001) Reflections on the concept of the central executive. In J. Andrade (Ed.), Working memory in perspective (pp. 240–260). Hove, East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Truscott, J.
(1996) The case against grammar correction in L2 writing classes. Language Learning, 46, 327–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1998) Noticing in second language acquisition: A critical review. Second Language Research, 14, 103–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) Optionality in second language acquisition: A generative, processing-oriented account. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 44, 311–330. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007a) The effect of error correction on learners’ ability to write accurately. Journal of Second Language Writing, 16, 255–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007b) Grammar teaching and the evidence: A response to Nassaji and Fotos (2004). International Journal of Foreign Language Teaching. Available at [URL]Google Scholar
(2015a) Consciousness and second language learning. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
(2015b) Consciousness in SLA: A modular perspective. Second Language Research, 31, 413–434. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) The effectiveness of error correction: Why do meta-analytic reviews produce such different answers? In Y-N. Leung (Ed.), Epoch making in English teaching and learning: Evolution, innovation, revolution. Taipei: Crane.Google Scholar
(2017) Modularity, working memory, and second language acquisition: A research program. Second Language Research, 33, 313–323. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) The place of instructed knowledge in speaking: A theoretical perspective. In Y.-N. Leung, J. Katchen, S.-Y. Hwang, & Y. Chen (Eds.), Reconceptualizing English language teaching and learning in the 21st century: A special monograph in memory of Professor Kai-Chong Cheung (pp. 107–121). Taipei: ETA-ROC and Crane.Google Scholar
Truscott, J., & Sharwood Smith, M.
(2004) Acquisition by processing: A modular approach to language development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Input, intake, and consciousness: The quest for a theoretical foundation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 497–528. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Representation, processing, and code-switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 903–916. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
in press). Theoretical frameworks in L2 acquisition. In J. W. Schwieter & A. Benati Eds. The Cambridge handbook of language learning Cambridge Cambridge University Press DOI logo
Tulving, E.
(1972) Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381–402). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
(2002) Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Turner, J. H.
(2000) On the origins of human emotions: A sociological inquiry into the evolution of human affect. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Umiltà, C.
(1988) The control operations of consciousness. In A. J. Marcel & E. Bisiach (Eds.), Consciousness in contemporary science (pp. 334–356). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Usrey, W. M., & Reid, R. C.
(1999) Synchronous activity in the visual system. Annual Review of Physiology, 61, 435–456. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vainikka, A., & Young-Scholten, M.
(1994) Direct access to X’-theory: Evidence from Korean and Turkish adults learning German. In T. Hoekstra & B. D. Schwartz (Eds.), Language acquisition studies in generative grammar (pp. 265–316). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996a) The early stages in adult L2 syntax: Additional evidence from Romance speakers. Second Language Research, 12, 140–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996b) Gradual development of L2 phrase structure. Second Language Research, 12, 7–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1998) Morphosyntactic triggers in adult SLA. In M.-L. Beck (Ed.), Morphology and its interfaces in second language knowledge (pp. 89–113). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valian, V.
(2015) Bilingualism and cognition: A focus on mechanisms. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 47–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vallar, G., & Baddeley, A. D.
(1984) Phonological short-term store, phonological processing and sentence comprehension: A neuropsychological case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1, 121–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1987) Phonological short-term store and sentence processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4, 417–438. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Boxtel, J. J. A., Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C.
(2010) Consciousness and attention: On sufficiency and necessity. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, Article 217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van den Noort, M. W. M. L., Bosch, P., & Hugdahl, K.
(2006) Foreign language proficiency and working memory capacity. European Psychologist, 11, 289–296. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van den Stock, J., Tamietto, M., Sorger, B., Pichon, S., Grezes, J., & de Gelder, B.
(2011) Cortico-subcortical visual, somatosensory and motor activations for perceiving dynamic whole-body emotional expressions with and without striate cortex (V1). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108, 16188–16193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Dijk, T. A.
(1977) Context and cognition: Knowledge frames and speech act comprehension. Journal of Pragmatics, 1, 211–232. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1980) The semantics and pragmatics of functional coherence in discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 4, 233–252.Google Scholar
(2001) Multidisciplinary CDA: A plea for diversity. In R. Wodak & M. Meyer (Eds.), Methods of critical discourse analysis (pp. 108–112). London: Sage.Google Scholar
(2006) Introduction: Discourse, interaction and cognition. Discourse Studies, 8(1), 5–7. Retrieved from [URL] DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Discourse and knowledge. A sociocognitive approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W.
(1983) Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
van Geert, P.
(2008) The dynamic systems approach in the study of L1 and L2 acquisition: An introduction. Modern Language Journal, 92, 179–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Hell, J., & de Groot, A.
(1998) Conceptual representation in bilingual memory: Effects of concreteness and cognate status in word association. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1, 193–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Hell, J. G., & Kroll, J. F.
(2013) Using electrophysiological measures to track the mapping of words to concepts in the bilingual brain: A focus on translation. In J. Altarriba & L. Isurin (Eds.), Memory, language, and bilingualism: Theoretical and applied approaches (pp. 126–160). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
van Hell, J. G., Litcofsky, K. A., & Ting, C. Y.
(2015) Intra-sentential code-switching: Cognitive and neural approaches. In J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing (pp. 459–482). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Heuven, W. J. B., & Dijkstra, A.
(2010) Language comprehension in the bilingual brain: fMRI and ERP support for psycholinguistic models. Brain Research Reviews, 64, 104–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Perniss, P., & Vinson, D.
(2014) Language as a multimodal phenomenon: Implications for language learning, processing and evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 369, 1–7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vigliocco, G., & Vinson, D. P.
(2007) Semantic representation. In G. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 195–215). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Vinson, D., Vigliocco, G., Cappa, S., & Siri, S.
(2003) The breakdown of semantic knowledge: Insights from a statistical model of meaning representation. Brain and Language, 86, 347–365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T.
(1984) Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829–854. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wegner, D. M.
(2002) The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wei, L.
(1998) The ‘why’ and ‘how’ questions in the analysis of conversational code-switching. In P. Auer (Ed.), Code-switching in conversation: Language, interaction and identity (pp. 156–176). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Wen, Z.
(2014) Theorizing and measuring working memory in first and second language research. Language Teaching, 47, 174–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Working memory in second language acquisition and processing: The phonological/executive model. In Z. Wen, M. B. Mota, & A. McNeill (Eds.), Working memory in second language acquisition and processing (pp. 41–62). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wen, Z., Mota, M. B., & McNeill, A.
(Eds.) (2015) Working memory in second language acquisition and processing. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Byers-Heinlein, K.
(2008) Bilingualism in infancy: First steps in perception and comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 144–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Westbrook, A., & Braver, T. S.
(2015) Cognitive effort: A neuroeconomic approach. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 395–415. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Westergaard, M., Mitrofanova, N., Mykhaylyk, R., & Rodina, Y.
(2017) Crosslinguistic influence in the acquisition of a third language: The Linguistic Proximity Model. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21, 666–682. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whalen, J. E., & Nash, M. R.
(1996) Hypnosis and dissociation: Theoretical, empirical, and clinical perspectives. In L. K. Michelson & W. J. Ray (Eds.), Handbook of dissociation: Theoretical, empirical, and clinical perspectives (pp. 191–206). New York, NY: Plenum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L.
(1991) Adverb placement in second language acquisition: Some effects of positive and negative evidence in the classroom. Second Language Research, 7, 133–161.Google Scholar
(1992a) Long and short verb movement in second language acquisition. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 37, 273–286. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1992b) On triggering data in L2 acquisition: A reply to Schwartz and Gubala-Ryzak. Second Language Research, 8, 120–137.Google Scholar
(2003) Second language acquisition and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whorf, B.
(1956) Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wiecki, T. V., & Frank, M. J.
(2013) A computational model of inhibitory control in frontal cortex and basal ganglia. Psychological Review, 120, 329–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wierzbicka, A.
(1996) Semantics: Primes and universals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Williams, J. N.
(2012) Working memory and SLA. In S. M. Gass and A. Mackey (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 427–441). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Williams, J.
(2015) Working memory in SLA research: Challenges and prospects. In Z. Wen, M. B. Mota, & A. McNeill (Eds.), Working memory in second language acquisition and processing (pp. 301–307). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Williams, S., & Hammarberg, B.
(1998) Language switches in L3 production: Implications for a polyglot speaking model. Applied Linguistics, 19, 295–333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wills, T. A., & Stoolmiller, M.
(2002) The role of self-control in early escalation of substance use: A time-varying analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 986–997. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, D., & Sperber, D.
(2004) Relevance theory. In L. R. Horn & G. Ward (Eds.), Handbook of pragmatics (pp. 607–632). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. A, Varela, C., & Remondes, M.
(2015) Phase organization of network computations. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 31, 250–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winke, P. M.
(2005) Individual differences in adult Chinese second language acquisition: The relationships among aptitude, memory and strategies for learning (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Georgetown University.Google Scholar
Wiss, F. C.
(1991) Aspects of the self (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Womelsdorf, T., Schoffelen, J.-M., Oostenveld, R., Singer, W., Desimone, R., Engel, A. K., & Fries, P.
(2007) Modulation of neuronal interactions through neuronal synchronization. Science, 316, 1609–1612. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wong, W., & Law, S.-P.
(2008) The relationship between semantic short-term memory and immediate serial recall of known and unknown words and nonwords: Data from two Chinese individuals with aphasia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 900–917. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Woumans, E., Santens, P., Sieben, A., Versijpt, J., Stevens, M., & Duyck, W.
(2014) Bilingualism delays clinical manifestation of Alzheimer’s disease. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 568–574. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G.
(2012) How reading in a second language protects your heart. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 6485–6489. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, C., Crain, S., Berwick, R., & Bolhuis, J.
(2017) The growth of language: Universal Grammar, experience, and principles of computation. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 81, 103–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zahodne, L. B., Schofield, P. W., Farrell, M. T., & Manly, J. J.
(2014) Bilingualism does not alter cognitive decline or dementia risk among Spanish-speaking immigrants. Neuropsychology, 28, 238–246. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zhou, J., Zhang, H., Ding, X., Shui, R., & Shen, M.
(2016) Object formation in visual working memory: Evidence from object-based attention. Cognition, 154, 95–101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R.
(2016) Situation models, mental simulations, and abstract concepts in discourse comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 1028–1034. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R., & Ross, B. H.
(2004) The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 35–62). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar