Part of
Give Constructions across Languages
Edited by Myriam Bouveret
[Constructional Approaches to Language 29] 2021
► pp. 7596
References (59)
References
Arnold, J., Wasow, T., Losongco, A., & Ginstrom, R. (2000). Heaviness vs. newness: The effects of complexity and information structure on constituent ordering. Language, 76, 28–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Behaghel, O. (1910). Beziehungen zwischen Umfang und Reihenfolge von Satzgliedern. Indogermanische Forschungen, 25, 110–142.Google Scholar
Boas, H. (2003). A constructional approach to resultatives. Stanford: CSLI.Google Scholar
Bresnan, J. (2007). Is syntactic knowledge probabilistic? Experiments with the English dative alternation. In S. Featherston, & W. Sternefeld (Eds.), Roots: Linguistics in search of its evidential base (pp. 76–96). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Bresnan, J., Cueni, A., Nikitina, T., & Baayen, R. H. (2007). Predicting the dative alternation. In G. Bouma, I. Kraemer, & Zwarts, J. (Eds.), Cognitive foundations of interpretation (pp. 69–94). Amsterdam: Royal Netherlands Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Bresnan, J., & Ford, M. (2010). Predicting syntax: Processing dative constructions in American and Australian varieties of English. Language, 86, 186–213.Google Scholar
Bresnan, J., & Hay, J. (2008). Gradient grammar: An effect of animacy on the syntax of give in New Zealand and American English. Lingua, 118, 245–259. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bresnan, J., & Nikitina, T. (2009). The gradience of the dative alternation. In L. Uyechi, & L. H. Wee (Eds.), Reality exploration and discovery: Pattern interaction in language and life (pp. 161–184). Stanford: CSLI.Google Scholar
Collins, P. (1995). The indirect object construction in English: An informational approach. Linguistics, 33, 35–49. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davies, M. (2008). The Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA): 560 million words, 1990-present. Available online at [URL].
(2018). The 14 Billion Word iWeb Corpus. Available online at [URL].
Dąbrowska, E. (1994). Some English equivalents of Polish dative constructions. Papers and Studies in Contrastive Linguistics, 29, 105–121.Google Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. (1997). Cognitive Semantics and the Polish dative. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Divjak, D. (2006). Ways of intending: A corpus-based Cognitive Linguistic approach to near-synonyms in Russian. In S. Th. Gries, & A. Stefanowitsch (Eds.), Corpora in Cognitive Linguistics: Corpus-based approaches to syntax and lexis (pp. 19–56). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Erteschik-Shir, N. (1979). Discourse constraints on dative movement. In T. Givon (Ed.), Discourse and syntax (pp. 441–467). New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garretson, G., O’Connor, M. C., Skarabela, B., & Hogan, M. (2004). Coding practices used in the project Optimal Typology of Determiner Phrases. [URL].
Geeraerts, D., Grondelaers, S., & Bakema, P. (1994). The structure of lexical variation: Meaning, naming, and context. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D., Grondelaers, S., & Speelman, D. (1999). Convergentie en divergentie in de Nederlandse woordenschat. Amsterdam: Meertens Instituut.Google Scholar
Glynn, D. (2004). Constructions at the crossroads. The place of construction grammar between field and frame. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 2, 197–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009). Polysemy, syntax, and variation: A usage-based method for Cognitive Semantics. In V. Evans, & S. Pourcel (Eds.), New directions in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 77–106). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010a). Synonymy, lexical fields, and grammatical constructions: A study in usage- based Cognitive Semantics. In H.-J. Schmid, & S. Handl (Eds.), Cognitive foundations of linguistic usage-patterns: Empirical studies (pp. 89–118). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010b). Testing the hypothesis: Objectivity and verification in usage-based Cognitive Semantics. In D. Glynn, & K. Fischer (Eds.), Quantitative Cognitive Semantics: Corpus-driven approaches (pp. 239–270). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014). The many uses of run: Corpus methods and Socio-Cognitive Semantics. In D. Glynn, & J. Robinson (Eds.), Corpus methods for semantics. Quantitative studies in polysemy and synonymy (pp. 117–144). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glynn, D., & Fischer, K. (Eds). (2010). Quantitative Cognitive Semantics: Corpus-driven approaches. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glynn, D., & Robinson, J. (Eds.). (2014). Corpus methods for semantics. Quantitative studies in polysemy and synonymy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. 2002. Surface generalizations: An alternative to alternations. Cognitive Linguistics, 13, 327–356. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006). Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Green, G. (1974). Semantics and syntactic regularity. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Gries St. T. (1999). Particle movement: A cognitive and functional approach. Cognitive Linguistics, 10, 105–145.Google Scholar
Gries, St. Th. (2003a). Multifactorial analysis in Corpus Linguistics: A study of particle placement. London: Continuum Press.Google Scholar
Gries, St. Th. (2003b). Towards a corpus-based identification of prototypical instances of constructions. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 1, 1–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006). Corpus-based methods and Cognitive Semantics: The many senses of to run . In St. Th. Gries, & A. Stefanowitsch (eds.), Corpora in Cognitive Linguistics: Corpus-based approaches to syntax and lexis (pp. 57–99). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gries, St. Th., & Stefanowitsch, A. (2004). Extending collostructional analysis: A corpus-based perspective on alternations. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 9, 97–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(Eds.). (2006). Corpora in Cognitive Linguistics: Corpus-based approaches to syntax and lexis. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heylen, K. (2005). A quantitative corpus study of German word order variation. In St. Kepser, & M. Reis (Eds.), Linguistic evidence: Empirical, theoretical and computational perspectives (pp. 241–264). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (2000). Applied logistic regression. New York: John Wiley & Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kilgarriff, A., Baisa, V., Buta, J., Jakubek, M., Kov, V., Michelfeit, J., Rychly, P., & Suchomel, V. (2014). The Sketch Engine: Ten years on. Lexicography, 1, 7–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kizach, J., & Mathiasen, T. (2013). The dative alternation in Danish and Polish – A learner’s perspective. Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 49(4), 487–507.Google Scholar
Krawczak, K., Fabiszak, M., & Hilpert, M. (2016). A corpus-based, cross-linguistic approach to mental predicates and their complementation: Performativity and descriptivity vis-à-vis boundedness and picturability. Folia Linguistica, 50(2), 475–506. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Michaelis, L. A. (2003). Headless Constructions and Coercion by Construction. In E. J. Francis, & L. A. Michaelis (Eds.), Mismatch: Form-function incongruity and the architecture of grammar (pp. 259–310). Stanford: CSLI.Google Scholar
Michaelis, L. A., & Ruppenhofer, J. (2001). Beyond alternations: A constructional model of the German applicative pattern. Stanford: CSLI.Google Scholar
Michaelis, L. A., & Hartwell, S. F. (2007). Lexical subjects and the conflation strategy. In N. Hedberg, & R. Zacharski (Eds.), Topics in the grammar-pragmatics interface: Papers in honor of Jeanette K. Gundel (pp. 19–48). Amsterdam: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. (1996). Give: A cognitive linguistic study. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition. The acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Prince, E. (1981). Toward a new taxonomy for given-new information. In P. Cole (Ed), Radical pragmatics (pp. 223–255). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2014). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL [URL]Google Scholar
Reddy, M. J. (1979). The conduit metaphor: A case of frame conflict in our language about language. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (pp. 284–310). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rudzka-Ostyn, B. (1992). Case relations in Cognitive Grammar. Some reflexive uses of the Polish dative. Leuvense Bijdragen, 81, 327–373.Google Scholar
(1996). The Polish dative. In W. Van Belle, & W. Van Langendonck, (Eds.), The dative: Descriptive studies (pp. 341–394). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siewierska, A. (1993). Syntactic weight vs. information structure and word order variation in Polish. Journal of Linguistics, 29, 233–265. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Speelman, D., & Geeraerts, D. (2009). Causes for causatives. The case of Dutch doen and laten . In T. Sanders, & E. Sweetser (Eds.), Causal categories in discourse and cognition (pp. 173–204). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Theijssen, D. L. (2012). Making choices. Modeling the English dative alternation. PhD dissertation, Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen.
Thompson, S. (1987). Information flow and ‘dative shift’ in English. Unpublished manuscript.
Wasow, T. (2002). Postverbal behavior. Stanford: CSLI.Google Scholar
Wasow, T., & J. Arnold. (2003). Post-verbal constituent ordering in English. In G. Rohdenburg, & B. Mondorf (Eds), Determinants of grammatical variation in English (pp. 119–154). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1986). The meaning of a case: A study of the Polish dative. In R. Brecht, & J. Levine (Eds.), Case in Slavic (pp. 386–426). Columbus: Slavica.Google Scholar
Williams, R.S. (1994). A statistical analysis of English double object alternation. Issues in Applied Linguistics, 5, 37–58.Google Scholar
Wolk, C., Bresnan, J., Rosenbach, A., & Szmrecsányi, B. (2013). Dative and genitive variability in Late Modern English: Exploring cross-constructional variation and change. Diachronica, 30, 382–419. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Glynn, Dylan & Olaf Mikkelsen
2024. Concrete constructions or messy mangroves? How modelling contextual effects on constructional alternations reflect theoretical assumptions of language structure. Linguistics Vanguard 10:s1  pp. 9 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 19 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.