References (41)
References
Babyonyshev, Maria, Jennifer Ganger, David Pesetsky & Kenneth Wexler. 2001. The maturation of grammatical principles: Evidence from Russian unaccusatives. Linguistic Inquiry 32(1). 1–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borer, Hagit & Kenneth Wexler. 1987. The maturation of syntax. In Tom Roeper & Edwin Williams (eds.), Parameter setting, 123–172. Dordrecht: Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
CETEMPúblico (Corpus de Extractos de Textos Electrónicos MCT/Público). [URL]
Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Michael Kenstowicz (ed.), Ken Hale: A life in language, 1–52. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Collins, Chris. 2005. A smuggling approach to the passive in English. Syntax 8(2). 81–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, Stephen, Rosalind Thornton & Keiko Musarugi. 2009. Capturing the evasive passive. Language Acquisition 16(2). 123–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demuth, Katherine. 1989. Maturation and the acquisition of the Sesotho passive. Language 65(1). 56–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duarte, Inês & Fátima Oliveira. 2010. Particípios resultativos. In Ana Maria Brito, Fátima Silva & João Veloso e Alexandra Fiéis (eds.), Textos Seleccionados do XXV Encontro da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística, 397–408. Porto: APL.Google Scholar
Embick, David. 2004. On the structure of resultative participles in English. Linguistic Inquiry 35(3). 355–392. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Estrela, Antónia. 2013. A aquisição da estrutura passiva em Português Europeu. Doctoral dissertation, Universidade Nova de Lisboa. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fox, Danny & Yosef Grodzinsky. 1998. Children’s passive: A view from the by-phrase. Linguistic Inquiry 29(2). 311–332. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Freitas, Maria João. 1997. Aquisição da Estrutura Silábica do Português Europeu. MA thesis, University of Lisbon.Google Scholar
Gavarró, Anna & Xavier Parramon. 2017. On the adjectival interpretation of passives in acquisition. Lingua 185. 11–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gehrke, Berit & Nino Grillo. 2007. Aspects on passives. In Sylvia Blaho, Eric Schoorlemmer & Luis Vicente (eds.), Proceedings of ConSOLE XIV, 121–142. Leiden: SOLE.Google Scholar
. 2009. How to become passive. In Kleanthes K. Grohmann (ed.), Explorations of phase theory: Features, arguments, and interpretation at the interfaces, 231–268. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirsch, Chris & Kenneth Wexler. 2006. Children’s passives and their resulting interpretation. The Proceedings of the inaugural conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition – North America, 125–136. University of Connecticut Occasional Papers in Linguistics 4.Google Scholar
Horgan, Dianne. 1975. Language development: A cross-methodological study. Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Hyams, Nina & William Snyder. 2005. Young children never smuggle: Reflexive clitics and the universal freezing hypothesis. Paper presented at the 30th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (BUCLD 30), Boston, MA, November 4–6.
Kiparsky, Paul. 2013. Towards a null theory of the passive. Lingua 125. 7–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koopman, Hilda & Dominique Sportiche. 1991. the position of subjects. Lingua 85. 211–259. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koring, Loes, Pim Mak, Iris Mulders & Eric Reuland. 2018. Processing intransitive verbs: How do children differ from adults?. Language Learning and Development 14(1). 72–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lima Júnior, João & Marina Augusto. 2015. Is ‘smuggling’ really necessary? The most recent analyses of passive sentences reconsidered in terms of phasehood and cyclic movement. ReVEL 9. 62–91.Google Scholar
Lima Júnior, João, Letícia Sicuro Corrêa & Marina Augusto. 2018. Favorable processing conditions in the production of passive sentences by Brazilian Portuguese-Speaking children. In Anna Gavarró (ed.), On the acquisition of the syntax of Romance, 91–115. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Manzini, Rita. 2017. Passive, smuggling and the by-phrase. In Nicholas LaCara, Keir Moulton & Anne-Michelle Tessier (eds.), A schrift to fest Kyle Johnson, 233–244. Linguistics Open Access Publications 1. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts Amherst.Google Scholar
Maratsos, Michael, Dana Fox, Judith Becker & Mary Anne Chalkley. 1985. Semantic restrictions in children’s passives. Cognition 19(2). 167–191. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Müller, Gereon. 1998. Incomplete category fronting. Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Brien, Karen, Elaine Grolla & Diane Lillo-Martin. 2006. Long passives are understood by young children. In David Bamman, Tatiana Magnitskaia & Colleen Zaller (eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (BUCLD 30), 441–451. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Oliva, Jesús & Kenneth Wexler. 2018. The acquisition of Spanish passives: The comparison between subject experiencer versus actional verbs and direct aspectual semantic evidence for the adjectival interpretation. In Anna Gavarró (ed.), On the acquisition of the syntax of Romance, 57–89. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Orfitelli, Robyn. 2012. Argument intervention in the acquisition of A-movement. Doctoral dissertation, UCLA.Google Scholar
Pierce, Amy. 1992. The acquisition of passives in Spanish and the question of A-chain maturation. Language Acquisition 1. 55–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Santos, Ana Lúcia, Aida Cardoso & Maria João Freitas. 2014. CEPLEXicon – A lexicon of child European Portuguese. ELRA ID: ELRA-L0094.Google Scholar
Santos, Ana Lúcia, Michel Généreux, Aida Cardoso, Celina Agostinho & Silvana Abalada. 2014. A corpus of European Portuguese child and child-directed speech. In Nicoletta Calzolari, Khalid Choukri, Thierry Declerck, Hrafn Loftsson, Bente Maegaard, Joseph Mariani, Asuncion Moreno, Jan Odijk & Stelios Piperidis (eds.), Proceedings of the Ninth International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2014), 1488–1491. Reykjavik: European Language Resources Association.Google Scholar
Snyder, William & Emma Nguyen. 2017. It’s hard to coerce: A unified account of raising-past-experiencers and passives in child English. Paper presented at the 13th Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition conference (GALA 13), Palma de Mallorca, Spain, September 7–9.
Snyder, William & Nina Hyams. 2015. Minimality effects in children’s passives. In Elisa Di Domenico, Cornelia Hamann & Simona Matteini (eds.), Structures, strategies and beyond: Essays in honour of Adriana Belletti, 343–368. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snyder, William, Nina Hyams & Paola Crisma. 1995. Romance auxiliary selection with reflexive clitics: Evidence for early knowledge of unaccusativity. In Eve V. Clark (ed.) Proceedings of the 26th Annual Child Language Research Forum, 127–136. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Stromswold, Karin. 1996. Does the VP-internal stage really exist?. Paper presented at the 21st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (BUCLD 26), Boston, MA, November 2–4.
Terzi, Arhonto & Kenneth Wexler. 2002. A-chains and s-homophones in children’s grammar: Evidence from Greek passives. North East Linguistics Society 32(2). 519–538.Google Scholar
Volpato, Francesca, Laura Verin & Anna Cardinaletti. 2016. The comprehension and production of verbal passives by Italian preschool-age children. Applied Psycholinguistics 36. 901–931. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wexler, Kenneth. 2004. Theory of phasal development: Perfection in child grammar. MITWPL 48. 159–209.Google Scholar
Weinberg, Amy. 1987. Comments on Borer and Wexler. In Thomas Roeper & Edwin Williams (eds.), Parameter setting, 173–187. Dordrecht: Reidel Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar