References (75)
References
Antonsen, Karoline & Romina Farhang. 2018. The effect of PP type on recursive modification. Poster presented at the meeting of the Canadian Linguistic Association, Regina, Saskatchewan, Canada, May 30-June 1.
Berko, Jean. 1958. The child’s learning of English morphology. WORD 14(2–3). 150–177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brooks, Patricia J. & Michael Tomasello. 1999. How children constrain their argument structure constructions. Language 75(4). 720–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, Roger & Jean Berko. 1960. Word association and the acquisition of grammar. Child Development 31(1). 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chierchia, Gennaro. 1998. Reference to kinds across languages. Natural Language Semantics 6. 339–405. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1957. Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton. ISBN: 3-11-017279-8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 1959. A Review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Language 35(1). 26–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 1981. Lectures on government and binding. Holland: Foris Publications.Google Scholar
. 1986. Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin, and use. Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
. 1995. The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
. 2001. Derivation by phase. In M. Kenstowicz (Ed.), Ken Hale: A life inlLanguage, 1–52. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve. 1995. The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, Sarita, Teresa Ukrainetz, Jennifer Hsu, Joan Kaderavek, Laura Justice, & Ronald Gillam. 2008. Noun phrase elaboration in children’s spoken stories. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 39. 145–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, Nicholas & Stephen C. Levinson. 2009. The myth of language universals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32. 429–492. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Everett, Daniel L. 2005. Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã. Current Anthropology 46. 621–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh. 2010. The evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, Janet. 1998. Unambiguous triggers. Linguistic Inquiry 29. 1–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Futrell, Richard, Laura Stearns, Daniel L. Everett, Steven T. Piantadosi & Edward Gibson. 2016. A corpus investigation of syntactic embedding in Pirahã. PLOS ONE 11. e0145289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, Lila R. 1990. The structural sources of verb meaning. Language Acquisition 1 (1). 3–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, Lila R., Kimberly Cassidy, Rebbeca Nappa, Anna Papafragou & John C. Trueswell. 2005. Hard words. Language learning and development 1(1). 23–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan. 1982. The resilience of recursion. In Eric Wanner & Lila R. Gleitman (eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art, 51–77. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Guasti, Maria Teresa, Anna Gavarró, Joke de Lange & Claudia Caprin. 2008. Article omission across child languages. Language Acquisition 15(2). 89–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Han, Chung-hye, Jeffrey Lidz & Julien Musolino. 2016. Endogenous sources of variation in language acquisition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113(4). 942–947. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, Marc D., Noam Chomsky & W. Tecumseh Fitch. 2002. The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298. 1569–1579. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heim, Irene. 1991. Artikel und Definitheit [Articles and definiteness]. In Armin von Stechow & Dieter Wunderlich (eds.), Semantik: Ein internationales Handbuch der zeitgenössischen Forschung, 487–535. Berlin: de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hunsicker, Dea & Susan Goldin-Meadow. 2012. Hierarchical structure in a self-created communication system. Language 88. 732–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leguin, Ursula. 2017. No time to spare. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Lidz, Jeff, Susan Waxman & Jennifer Freedman. 2003. What infants know about syntax but couldn’t have learned. Cognition 89(3). B65–B73. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, David. 1991. How to set parameters: Arguments from language change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Limbach, Maxi & Dani Adone. 2010. Language acquisition of recursive possessives in English. Proceedings of the Boston University Conference on Language Development 34. 281–90.Google Scholar
Longobardi, Giuseppe. 2018. Principles, parameters, and schemata: A radically underspecified UG. Linguistic Analysis 41 (3). 517–557.Google Scholar
Marcus, Gary F., Sugumaran Vijayan, S. Bandi Rao & Peter M. Vishton. 1999. Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science 283(5398). 77–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Markman, Ellen M. & Gwin F. Wachtel. 1988. Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology 20. 121–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moreira-Rodríguez, Antonia. 2006. ‘The book on the table’, ‘The nan in the moon’: Post-modification of nouns by preposition + noun in English and Castilian. Bulletin of Spanish Studies 83(1). 53–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nevins, Andrew, David Pesetsky & Cilene Rodrigues. 2009. Pirahã exceptionality: A reassessment. Language 85. 355–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ninio, Anat. 2011. Syntactic development, its input and output. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pannemann, Maren. 2007. DP acquisition as structure unravelling. Amsterdam: Universiteit van Amsterdam dissertation.
Paradis, Johanne & Fred Genesee. 1996. Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 18 (1). 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pearl, Lisa & Jeff Lidz. 2013. Parameters in language acquisition. In Cedruc Boeckx & Kleanthes K. Grohmann (eds.), Cambridge handbook of biolinguistics, 129–159. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pearson, Barbara Zurer, Sylvia C. Fernandez, & D. Kimbrough Oller. 1993. Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers. Language Learning 43 (1). 93–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, Ana T., & Anny P. Castilla-Earls. 2016. Adquisición del español como lengua materna. In Javier Gutierrez-Rexach (ed.), Enciclopedia de lingüística hispánica, 213–230. New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, Ana T., Anny Castilla-Earls, Susana Béjar & Diane Massam. 2012. Elmo’s sister’s ball: The problem of acquiring nominal recursion. Language Acquisition 19. 301–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, Ana T., Tyler Peterson, Anny Castilla-Earls, Susana Béjar, Diane Massam & Yves Roberge. 2018. The acquisition of recursive modification in NPs. Language 94. 332–359. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, Ana T., Erin Pettibone & Anny Castilla-Earls. 2017. Down two steps: Are bilinguals delayed in the acquisition of recursively embedded PPs? Matraga (Aquisição da Linguagem) 24(41). 393–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, Ana T. & Yves Roberge. 2014. Development of noun phrase complexity in children. Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. Insight Grant Program. SSHRC: 435-2014-2000.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, Ana T., Yves Roberge, Alex Lowles, & Petra Schulz. 2021. Structural diversity does not affect the acquisition of recursive embedding: The case of possession in German. Language Acquisition. DOI logo
Pérez-Leroux, Ana T., Castilla-Earls, María Fernanda Lara Díaz, & Erin Pettibone. In preparation. La adquisición de estructuras nominales complejas. University of Toronto.
Pesetsky, David. 2009. Against taking linguistic diversity at face value. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32(5). 462–465. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pettibone, Erin. In preparation. The acquisition of (non)restrictive adjectives. Toronto, Canada: University of Toronto dissertation.
Picallo, Carme. 2012. Structure of the noun phrase. In Jose Ignacio Hualde, Antxon Olarrea & Erin O’Rourke (eds), The handbook of Hispanic linguistics, 263–284. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pine, Julien M. & Elena V. M. Lieven. 1997. Slot and frame patterns and the development of the determiner category. Applied Psycholinguistics 18. 123–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pirvulescu, Mihaela, Yves Roberge, Danielle Thomas, Ana T. Pérez- Leroux, & Nelleke Strik. 2014. Bilingual effects: Exploring object omission in pronominal languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 17(3). 495–510. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Stephen. 1984. Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, Ana T. & Yves Roberge. 2018. A way into recursion. In Hollebrandse, Bart, Jaieun Kim, Ana T. Pérez-Leroux, & Petra Schulz (Eds.), UMOP 41: Thoughts on Mind and Grammar (T.O.M. and grammar): A Festschrift in Honor of Tom Roeper, 109–122. Amherst, MA: GLSA.Google Scholar
Roberge, Yves, Pérez-Leroux, Ana T. & Anna Frolova. 2018. On recursive modification in L1 French. Languages 3 (6). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roeper, Tom. 2011. The acquisition of recursion: How formalism articulates the child’s path. Biolinguistics 5. 57–86Google Scholar
Roeper, Tom & William Snyder. 2005. Language learnability and the forms of recursion. In Anna Maria Di Sciullo (ed.), UG and external systems: Language, brain and computation, 155–169. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roeper, Tom & Jill G. de Villiers. 2011. The acquisition path for wh-questions. In Jill G. de Villiers & Tom Roeper (eds.), Handbook of generative approaches to language acquisition, 189–246. Dordrecht: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saffran, Jenny R., Richard N. Aslin & Elissa L. Newport. 1996. Statistical learning by 8-month old infants. Science 274. 1926–1928. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sauerland, Uli. 2006. On the semantic markedness of phi features. In Daniel Habour, David Adger & Susana Béjar (eds.), Phi-theory: Phi features across modules and interfaces, 57–82. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sauerland, Uli & Hans-Martin Gärtner. 2007. Interfaces + recursion = language? Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Saussure, Ferdinand de. 1916. Cours de linguistique générale. Paris: Ed. Payot.Google Scholar
Sneed German, Elisa. 2008. Input in the acquisition of genericity. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University dissertation.
Snyder, William. 2007. Child language: The parametric approach. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Snyder, William, Ann Senghas & Kelly Ihnman. 2001. Agreement morphology and the acquisition of noun-drop in Spanish. Language Acquisition 9(2).157–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Syrett, Kristen & Jeff Lidz. 2009. QR in child grammar: Evidence from antecedent-contained deletion. Language Acquisition 16. 67–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thordardottir, Elin. 2014. The typical development of simultaneous bilinguals. In Johanne Paradis and Therese Grüter (eds.), Input and experience in bilingual development, 141–160. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 2000. Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 74. 209–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, Sharon. 2014. Comparing the role of input in bilingual acquisition across domains. In Johanne Paradis & Therese Grüter (eds.), Input and experience in bilingual development, 181–201. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valian, Virginia, Stephanie Solt & John Stewart. 2009. Abstract categories or limited-scope formulae: The case of children’s determiners. Journal of Child Language 36(4). 743–778. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Widmer, Manuel, Sandra Auderset, Johanna Nichols, Paul Widmer & Balthasar Bickel. 2017. NP recursion over time: Evidence from Indo-European. Language 93. 799–826. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, Charles. 2002. Knowledge and learning in natural language. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
. 2004. Universal Grammar, statistics, or both. Trends in Cognitive Science 8(10). 449–454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2015. For and against frequencies. Journal of Child Language 42(2). 287–293. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2016. The price of linguistic productivity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar