Article published In:
Concentric
Vol. 46:1 (2020) ► pp.6694
References
Champollion, Lucas
2016Homogeneity in donkey sentences. Proceedings of the 26th Semantics and Linguistic Theory Conference, SALT, ed. by Mary Moroney, Carol-Rose Little, Jacob Collard and Dan Burgdorf, 684–704. Austin, TX: University of Texas. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cheng, Lisa Lai-Shen, and C.-T. James Huang
1996Two types of donkey sentences. Natural Language Semantics 41:121–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cheung, Candice Chi Hang
2007The syntax and semantics of bare conditionals in Chinese. Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung 111, ed. by Estela Puig-Waldmüller, 150–164. Barcelona: Universitat Pompeu Fabra.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam
1986Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin and Use. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Cohen, Ariel
2001Relative readings of many, often, and generics. Natural Language Semantics 9.1:41–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cook, Vivian James
1988Chomsky’s Universal Grammar: An Introduction. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Crain, Stephen, Rosalind Thornton, Carole Boster, Laura Conway, Diane Lillo-Martin, and Elaine Woodams
2009Quantification without quantification. Language Acquisition 5.2:83–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dekker, Paul
2001On if and only. Proceedings of the 11th Semantics and Linguistic Theory Conference, SALT, ed. by Rachel Hastings, Brendan Jackson and Zsofia Zvolenszky, 114–133. New York: New York University. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeVault, David, and Matthew Stone
2004Interpreting vague utterances in context. Proceedings of the 20th International Conference on Computational Linguistics, COLING, ed. by Lothar Lemnitzer, Detmar Meurers and Erhard Hinrichs, 1247–1253. Geneva, Switzerland: University of Geneva. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foppolo, Francesca
2009The puzzle of donkey anaphora resolution. Proceedings of the 38th Annual Meeting of the North East Linguistic Society, ed. by Martin Walkow and Muhammad Abdurrahman, 297–310. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts.Google Scholar
Gelman, Susan A., and Henry M. Wellman
1991Insides and essences: Early understandings of the non-obvious. Cognition 38.3:213–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geurts, Bart
2002Donkey business. Linguistics and Philosophy 25.2:129–156. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2003Quantifying kids. Language Acquisition 111:197–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, Alison
1988Conceptual and semantic development as theory change: The case of object permanence. Mind & Language 3.3:197–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, Alison, and Andrew N. Meltzoff
1997Words, Thoughts, and Theories. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Grosz, Patrick G., Pritty Patel-Grosz, Evelina Fedorenko, and Edward Gibson
2014Constraints on donkey pronouns. Journal of Semantics 32.4:619–648. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin
1997Indefinite Pronouns. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Herburger, Elena
1997Focus and weak noun phrases. Natural Language Semantics 5.1:53–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2015Conditional perfection: The truth and the whole truth. Proceedings of the 25th Semantics and Linguistic Theory Conference, SALT, ed. by Sarah D’Antonio, Mary Moroney and Carol Rose Little, 615–635. Stanford, CA: Stanford University.Google Scholar
Huang, C.-T. James
1984On the distribution and reference of empty pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry 15.4:531–574.Google Scholar
Inhelder, Bärbel, and Jean Piaget
1958The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence: An Essay on the Construction of Formal Operational Structures. New York: Basic Books. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ireri, Anthony M., Daniel M. Mukuni, Philomena N. Mathuvi, Amos M. Njagi, and Njagi I. Karugu
2012An overview of major biological and contextual factors in language acquisition. American Journal of Linguistics 1.3:33–39.Google Scholar
Kanazawa, Makoto
1994Weak vs. strong readings of donkey sentences and monotonicity inferences in a dynamic setting. Linguistics and Philosophy 17.2:109–158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keil, Frank C.
1989Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Krifka, Manfred
1996Pragmatic strengthening in plural predications and donkey sentences. Proceedings of the 6th Semantics and Linguistic Theory Conference, SALT, ed. by Teresa Galloway and Justin Spence, 136–153. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lin, Keng-yu, and Shiao-hui Chan
2019When senses meet functions: An amodal stage in conceptual processing. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 31.1:64–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ni, Weijia
1987Empty topics in Chinese. UConn Working Papers in Linguistics 11. Storrs, CT: University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
Pan, Haihua, and Yan Jiang
1997NP interpretation and donkey sentences in Chinese. Paper presented at the Workshop on Interface Strategies in Chinese, Cornell University, Ithaca.
Pearson, Barbara Zurer, and Peter de Villiers
2005Child language acquisition: Discourse, narrative, and pragmatics. Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics (2nd edition), ed. by Keith Brown. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Piaget, Jean
1936Origins of Intelligence in the Child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Prévost, Philippe, and Johanne Paradis
Tannen, Deborah
1982Spoken and Written Language: Exploring Orality and Literacy. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
Thuan, Tran, and Benjamin Bruening
2013Wh-phrases as indefinites: A Vietnamese perspective. Linguistics of Vietnamese: An International Survey, ed. by Daniel Hole and Elisabeth Löbel, 217–241. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, Xin
2007Guanyu luziju de jidian yiwen yu sikao [Some questions and reflections about donkey sentences]. Yuwen Xuekan 21:69–72.Google Scholar
Yatsushiro, Kazuko
2008Quantifier acquisition: Presuppositions of “every”. Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung 121, ed. by Atle Grønn, 663–677. Oslo: University of Oslo.Google Scholar