Has language developed to serve as a system of communication or one of thought? While language is clearly an excellent tool for communication, the possible contribution of higher order cognitive processes other than language to communication may provide insights on how we think about language evolution. Studies show that bilingualism improves communication skills, possibly due to boosting domain general processes, thus suggesting a divide between communication and formal language. However, to date little attention has been paid to bilingual atypical child populations presenting asymmetric developmental delays in language and communication skills. In this study, we explore the language-communication divide in bilingual and monolingual children diagnosed with High Functioning Autism (HFA), displaying primary communication difficulties, and those with Specific Language Impairment (SLI), displaying primary language difficulties. Children were tested on language, cognitive control, Theory of Mind (ToM), social comprehension and verbal communication. Results demonstrate that bilingual children with HFA exhibit enhanced cognitive control, ToM and social comprehension, which in turn improve their verbal communication as compared to their monolingual peers with HFA. On the other hand, no boost emerges in bilingual children with SLI, be it in cognitive control or ToM, though a weaker advantage (relative to the children with ASD) can be observed in their verbal communication skills, with significant mediation from social comprehension. The overall evidence suggests that language, though useful for communication, is not sufficient for communication, and may arguably not have evolved primarily for communicative purposes.
Alloway, T. P., & Gathercole, S. E. (2005). Working memory and short-term sentence recall in young children. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,
17
(2), 207–220.
American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5™ (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc..
Andreou, M. (2015). The effects of bilingualism on verbal and non verbal cognition: the micro- and macro-structure of narratives in the weak and the dominant language of the bilingual child. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece.
Andrés-Roqueta, C., & Katsos, N. (2017). The Contribution of Grammar, Vocabulary and Theory of Mind in Pragmatic Language Competence in Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders. Frontiers in Psychology,
8
1, 996.
Archibald, L. M., 7 Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
41
(6), 675–93.
Asoulin, E. (2016). Language as an instrument of thought. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics,
1
(1), 1–23.
Atonius, J. (2018). The Analysis of Illocutionary Acts in Narrative Text of Eleventh Grade Students at SMA Negeri 5 Medan. Doctoral dissertation, Nommensen HKBP University, Indonesia.
Bal, V. H., Wilkinson, E., White, L. C., Law, J. K., THE SPARK Consortium, Feliciano, P., & Chung, W. K. (2021). Early Pandemic Experiences of Autistic Adults: Predictors of Psychological Distress. Autism Research,
14
(6), 1209–1219.
Bang, J., & Nadig, A. (2015). Learning Language in Autism: Maternal Linguistic Input Contributes to Later Vocabulary. Autism Research,
8
(2), 214–223.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
51
(6), 1173–1182.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. The MIT Press.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind?”Cognition,
21
1, 37–46.
Bedore, L. M., & Peña, E. D. (2008). Assessment of Bilingual Children for Identification of Language Impairment: Current Findings and Implications for Practice. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism,
11
(1), 1–29.
Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., García, M., Cortez, C. (2005). Conceptual versus monolingual scoring: When does it make a difference?Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools,
36
(3), 188–200.
Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
10
(6), 258–264.
Berwick, R. C., & Chomsky, N. (2016). Why Only us. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development,
70
(3), 636–644.
Bialystok, E. (2011). Reshaping the mind: the benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology,
65
1, 229–235.
Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child development perspectives,
9
(2), 117–121.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition,
4
1, 859–873.
Bialystok, E., Poarch, G., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M. (2014). Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and aging,
29
(3), 696–705.
Bialystok, E., & Senman, L. (2004). Executive Processes in Appearance-Reality Tasks: The Role of Inhibition of Attention and Symbolic Representation. Child Development,
75
(2), 562–579.
Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition,
112
(3), 494–500.
Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2005). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology: I: Generativity. Autism,
9
(1), 7–27.
Bishop, D. V. M., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., Greenhalgh, T., & the catalise consortium (2017). CATALISE: a multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development. Phase 2. Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
58
1, 1068–1080.
Blom, E., & Boerma, T. (2016). Why do children with language impairment have difficulties with narrative macrostructure?Research in Developmental Disabilities,
55
1, 301–11.
Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meaning of words. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and language disorders. New York: Wiley.
Boerma, T., & Blom, E. (2020). Effects of developmental language disorder and bilingualism on children’s executive functioning: A longitudinal study. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
107
1, 103782.
Boerma, E., Leseman, P., Timmermeister, M., Wijnen, F. & Blom, E. (2016). Narrative abilities of monolingual and bilingual children with and without language impairment: implications for clinical practice. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
51
(6), 626–638.
Bohnacker, U., & Gagarina, N. (2019). Background on MAIN –Revised, how to use it and adapt it to other languages. ZAS Papers in Linguistics,
63
1, iv–xii.
Bolhuis, J. J., Tattersall, I., Chomsky, N., & Berwick, R. C. (2014) How Could Language Have Evolved?PLoS Biology,
12
(8), e1001934.
Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2001). Non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
36
(4), 421–432.
Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Autism, primary pragmatic difficulties, and specific language impairment: can we distinguish them using psycholinguistic markers?Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
45
(8), 515–524.
Bouvet, L., Rousset, S., Valdois, S., & Donnadieu, S. (2011). Global precedence effect in audition and vision: evidence for similar cognitive styles across modalities. Acta Psychologica,
138
(2), 329–335.
Brock, J., Norbury, C., Einav, S. & Nation, K. (2008). Do individuals with autism process words in context?Cognition,
108
1, 896–904.
Brown-Schmidt, S. (2009). The role of executive function in perspective taking during online language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,
16
1, 893–900.
Bruyer, R., & Scailquin, J. C. (2000). The fate of global precedence with age. Experimental Aging Research,
26
(4), 285–314.
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science,
11
(2), 282–298.
Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: an investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology,
33
(5), 781–789.
Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2005). Constraints and preadaptations in the earliest stages of language evolution. The Linguistic Review,
22
(2–4), 135–159.
Chondrogianni, V., Andreou, M., Nerantzini, M., Varlokosta, S., & Tsimpli, I. M. (2013). The Greek Sentence Repetition Task. COST Action IS0804.
Conti-Ramsden, G., Simkin, Z., & Botting, N. (2006). The prevalence of autistic spectrum disorders in adolescents with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
47
(6), 621–628.
Cormack, A., & Smith, N. (2002). Compositionality, copy theory, and control. University College London Working Papers in Linguistics,
14
1, 355–373.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1997). The Modular Nature of Human intelligence. In A. Scheibel & J. Schopf (Eds.), The Origin and Evolution of Intelligence (pp. 71–101). Jones and Bartlett publishers, Sudbury, MA.
Dahlgren, S., & Sandberg, A. D. (2008). Referential communication in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism,
12
(4), 335–348.
Davies, C., Andrés-Roqueta, C., & Norbury, C. F. (2016). Referring expressions and structural language abilities in children with specific language impairment: A pragmatic tolerance account. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
144
1, 98–113.
Dawson, G., & Fernald, M. (1987). Perspective-taking ability and its relationship to the social behavior of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
17
1, 487–498.
De Cat, C. (2013). Egocentric definiteness errors and perspective evaluation in preschool children. Journal of Pragmatics,
56
1, 58–69.
De Houwer, A., Bornstein, M. H., & Putnick, D. L. (2014). A bilingual-monolingual comparison of young children’s vocabulary size: Evidence from comprehension and production. Applied Psycholinguistics,
35
(6), 1189–1211.
Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Quintana, D. S., Naismith, S. L., Song, Y., Pye, J. E., Hickie, I., & Guastella, A. J. (2018). Autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of executive function. Molecular psychiatry,
23
(5), 1198–1204.
Díaz, V. (2021). Minds in action: Evidence that linguistic diversity helps children build a theory of mind. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1–11.
Diaz, V., & Farrar, M. J. (2017). The missing explanation of the false-belief advantage in bilingual children: a longitudinal study. Developmental Science,
21
(4), e12594.
Dukette, D., & Stiles, J. (1996). Children’s analysis of hierarchical patterns: Evidence from a similarity judgment task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
63
1, 103–140.
Durrleman, S., Baldimtsi, E., Peristeri, E., & Tsimpli, I. M. (to appear). Bilingualism matters: a study of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Developmental Language Disorder. In K. Grohmann (Ed.), Multifaceted Multilingualism: Language, Cognition, and Communication (pp. xx–xx). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Engel de Abreu, P. M., Cruz-Santos, A., & Puglisi, M. L. (2014). Specific language impairment in language-minority children from low-income families. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
49
(6), 736–747.
Fan, S. P., Liberman, Z., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2015). The exposure advantage: Early exposure to a multilingual environment promotes effective communication. Psychological Science,
26
1, 1090–1097.
Farhadian, M., Abdullah, R., Mansor, M., Redzuan, M., Gazanizadand, N., & Kumar, V. (2010). Theory of mind in bilingual and monolingual preschool children. The Journal of Psychology,
1
1, 39–46.
Fisher, N., Happé, F., & Dunn, J. (2005). The relationship between vocabulary, grammar, and false belief task performance in children with autistic spectrum disorders and children with moderate learning difficulties. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
46
(4), 409–419.
Fleckstein, A., Prévost, P., Tuller, L., Sizaret, E., & Zebib, R. (2018). How to identify SLI in bilingual children: a study on sentence repetition in French. Language Acquisition,
25
1, 85–101.
Fodor, J. A. (2008). LOT 2: The Language of Thought Revisited: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford, Oxford University Press.
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: explaining the enigma. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Frith, U. (2012). Why we need cognitive explanations of autism. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology,
65
(11), 2073–2092.
Frith, C., Frith, U. (2012). Mechanisms of social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology,
63
1, 287–313.
Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994a). Autism: beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition,
50
(1–3), 115–132.
Frith, U., Happé, F. (1994b). Language and communication in autistic disorders. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1994 Oct29;346(1315):97–104.
Gagarina, N., Klop, D., Kunnari, S., Tantele, K., Välimaa, T., Balčiūnienė, I., Bohnacker, U., & Walters, J. (2012). MAIN: Multilingual assessment instrument for narratives. ZAS Papers in Linguistics,
56
1, 1–140.
Gagarina, N., Klop, D., Kunnari, S., Tantele, K., Välimaa, T., Balčiūnienė, I., Bohnacker, U., & Walters, J. (2015). Assessment of narrative abilities in bilingual children. In Armon-Lotem, S., Jong, J. D. & Meir, N. (Eds.), Assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment (pp. 243–276). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Gagarina, N., Klop, D., Kunnari, S., Tantele, K., Välimaa, T., Bohnacker, U., & Walters, J. (2019). MAIN: Multilingual Assessment Instrument for Narratives. Revised version. ZAS Papers in Linguistics,
63
1.
Garfield, J. L., Peterson, C. C., & Perry, T. (2001). Social cognition, language acquisition and the development of the theory of mind. Mind & Language,
16
(5), 494–541.
Georgas, J., Paraskevopoulos, I. N., Besevegis, E., Giannitsas, N., & Mylonas, K. (2003). Ελληνικό WISC-III: Wechsler κλίμακες νοημοσύνης για παιδιά [Greek WISC-III: Wechsler intelligence scale for children]. Athens: Ellinika Grammata.
Goetz, P. (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,
6
1, 1–15.
Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., & Nadig, A. S. (2017). Can Bilingualism Mitigate Set-Shifting Difficulties in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders?Child Development,
90
(4), 1043–1060.
Grandgeorge, M., Hausberger, M., Tordjman, S., Deleau, M., Lazartigues, A., & Lemonnier, E. (2009). Environmental Factors Influence Language Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. PLoS ONE,
4
(4), e4683.
Gray, S. (2006). The Relationship Between Phonological Memory, Receptive Vocabulary, and Fast Mapping in Young Children With Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
49
(5), 955–969.
Greenberg, A., Bellana, B., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Perspective-taking ability in bilingual children: Extending advantages in executive control to spatial reasoning. Cognitive development,
28
(1), 41–50.
Gross, M., Buac, M., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2014). Conceptual scoring of receptive and expressive vocabulary measures in simultaneous and sequential bilingual children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,
23
(4), 574–586.
Guy, M. W., Reynolds, G. D., & Zhang, D. (2013). Visual attention to global and local stimulus properties in six-month-old infants: Individual differences and event-related potentials. Child Development,
84
1, 1392–1406.
Han, S., & Lee, K. (2013). Cognitive and affective perspective-taking ability of young bilinguals in South Korea. Child Studies in Asia-Pacific Contexts,
3
(1), 69–80.
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?Science,
298
(5598), 1569–1579.
Hudry, K., Leadbitter, K., Temple, K., Slonims, V., McConachie, H., Aldred, C., Howlin, P., Charman, T., & the PACT Consortium (2010). Preschoolers with autism show greater impairment in receptive compared with expressive language abilities. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
45
(6), 681–690.
Ikizer, E., & Ramirez-Esparza, N. (2018). Bilinguals’ social flexibility. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,
21
(5), 957–969.
Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2007). Integrating simulation and theory of mind: From self to social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
11
(5), 194–196.
Kimchi, R., Hadad, B., Behrmann, M., & Palmer, S. E. (2005). Microgenesis and ontogenesis of perceptual organization: evidence from global and local processing of hierarchical patterns. Psychological Science,
16
(4), 282–290.
Kimura, D. (1993). Neuromotor mechanisms in human communication. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Kissine, M., Luffin, X., Aiad, F., Bourourou, R., Deliens, G., & Gaddour, N. (2019). Noncolloquial Arabic in Tunisian Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Possible Instance of Language Acquisition in a Noninteractive Context. Language Learning,
69
(1), 44–70.
Kovács, A. M. (2009). Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false-belief reasoning. Developmental Science,
12
(1), 48–54.
Kover, S. T., McDuffie, A. S., Hagerman, R. J., & Abbeduto, L. (2013). Receptive vocabulary in boys with autism spectrum disorder: cross-sectional developmental trajectories. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
43
(11), 2696–709.
Koyama, T., Tachimori, H., Osada, H., Takeda, T., & Kurita, H. (2007). Cognitive and symptom profiles in Asperger’s syndrome and high-functioning autism. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences,
61
1, 99–104.
Krizman, J., Marian, V., Shook, A., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2012). Subcortical encoding of sound is enhanced in bilinguals and relates to executive function advantages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
109
(20) 7877–7881.
Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Leonard, L. B., Eyer, J. A., Bedore, L. M., & Grela, B. G. (1997). Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme difficulties of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
40
(4), 741–753.
Liberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2017). Exposure to multiple languages enhances communication skills in infancy. Developmental science,
20
(1),
Losh, M., & Capps, L. (2003). Narrative ability in high-functioning children with autism or Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
33
(3), 239–251.
Loukusa, S., Mäkinen, L., Kuusikko-Gauffin, S., Ebeling, H., & Moilanen, I. (2014). Theory of mind and emotion recognition skills in children with specific language impairment, autism spectrum disorder and typical development: Group differences and connection to knowledge of grammatical morphology, word-finding abilities and verbal working memory. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
49
1, 498–507.
Manson, J., Bryant, G., Gervais, M., & Kline, M. (2013). Convergence of speech rate in conversation predicts cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior,
34
(6), 419–426.
Marinis, T., & Sharon Armon-Lotem, S. (2015). Sentence repetition. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment (pp. 95–124). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2003). Weak coherence, no theory of mind, or executive dysfunction? Solving the puzzle of pragmatic disorders. Brain and Language,
85
1, 451–466.
Mattheoudakis, M., Chatzidaki, A., Maligkoudi, C., & Agathopoulou, A. (2016). Family and School Language Input: Their role in bilingual children’s vocabulary development. Journal of Applied Linguistics,
31
1, 49–69.
Matthews, D., Biney, H., & Abbot-Smith, K. (2018). Individual Differences in Children’s Pragmatic Ability: A Review of Associations with Formal Language, Social Cognition, and Executive Functions. Language Learning and Development,
14
(3), 186–223.
Maynard-Smith, J., & Szathmáry, E. (1995). The Major Transitions in Evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.
McEvoy, R. E., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1993). Executive function and social communication deficits in young autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
34
1, 563–578.
Meir, N., & Novogrodsky, R. (2019). Prerequisites of Third-Person Pronoun Use in Monolingual and Bilingual Children With Autism and Typical Language Development. Frontiers in Psychology.
Meir, N., & Novogrodsky, R. (2020). Syntactic abilities and verbal memory in monolingual and bilingual children with High Functioning Autism (HFA). First Language,
40
(4), 341–366.
Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
114
1, 187–202.
Nadig, A., Vivanti, G., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Adaptation of object descriptions to a partner under increasing communicative demands: a comparison of children with and without autism. Autism Research,
2
1, 334–347.
Navon, D. (1981). The forest revisited: More on global precedence. Psychological Research,
43
1, 1–32.
Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D. (2002). Inferential processing and story recall in children with communication problems: A comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
37
(3), 227–251.
Norrick, N. R. (2015). Narrative illocutionary acts direct and indirect. Journal of Pragmatics,
86
1, 94–99.
Nowell, K. P., Schanding, G. T. S., Kanne, S. M., & Goin-Kochel, R. P. (2015). Cognitive profiles in youth with autism spectrum disorder: an investigation of base rate discrepancies using the Differential Ability Scales – Second Edition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
45
1, 1978–1988.
O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
27
1, 719–730.
Papadopoulos, D. (2021). Mothers’ Experiences and Challenges Raising a Child with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Qualitative Study. Brain Sciences,
11
1, 309.
Pappas, M. A., Papoutsi, C., & Drigas, A. S. (2018). Policies, Practices, and Attitudes toward Inclusive Education: The Case of Greece. Social Sciences,
7
(6), 90.
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning,
43
1, 93–120.
Peristeri, E., Andreou, M., & Tsimpli, I. M. (2017). Syntactic and Story Structure Complexity in the Narratives of High- and Low-Language Ability Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Frontiers in Psychology.
Peristeri, E., Baldimtsi, E., Andreou, M., & Tsimpli, I. M. (2020). The Impact of Bilingualism on the Narrative Ability and the Executive Functions of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders,
85
1, 105999.
Peristeri, E., Baldimtsi, E., Tsimpli, I. M., & Durrleman, S. (2019). Bilingualism Effects in Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Metalinguistic Awareness, Executive Functions, and False-Belief Reasoning. In M. M. Brown, & B. Dailey (Eds.), BUCLD 43: Proceedings of the 43rd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 549–560). Cascadilla Press.
Peristeri, E., Baldimtsi, E., Vogelzang, M., Tsimpli, I. M., & Durrleman, S. (2021). The cognitive benefits of bilingualism in autism spectrum disorder: Is theory of mind boosted and by which underlying factors?Autism Research,
14
(8), 1695–1709.
Peristeri, E., Tsimpli, I. M., & Andreou, M. (2016). Object clitic production in monolingual and bilingual children with Specific Language Impairment: a comparison between elicited production and narratives. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism,
7
(3/4), 394–430.
Peristeri, E., Tsimpli, I. M., & Durrleman, S. (2021). Positive Effects of Bilingualism on Social Cognition in Autism Spectrum Disorder: a Study of Social Exclusion and Theory of Mind. In D. Dionne & L. A. Vidal Covas (Eds.), BUCLD 45: Proceedings of the 44th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 626–638). Cascadilla Press.
Pijnacker, J., Geurts, B., van Lambalgen, M., Buitelaar, J., Hagoort, P. (2010). Exceptions and anomalies: an ERP study on context sensitivity in autism. Neuropsychologia,
48
1, 2940–2951.
Pijnacker, J., Hagoort, P., Buitelaar, J., Teunisse, J. P., & Geurts, B. (2009). Pragmatic Inferences in High-Functioning Adults with Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
39
1, 607.
Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
13
(4), 707–727.
Pinker, S., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: what’s special about it?Cognition,
95
(2), 201–236.
Prentza, A., Tafiadis, D., Chondrogianni, V., & Tsimpli, I. M. (2022). Validation of a Greek Sentence Repetition task with typically developing monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.
Reed, T., & Peterson, C. (1990). A Comparative Study of Autistic Subjects’ Performance at Two Levels of Visual and Cognitive Perspective Taking. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
20
1, 555–567.
Renfrew, C. (1997). The renfrew language scales – Bus story test: A test of narrative speech/Word finding vocabulary test/Action picture test (4th ed.). Speechmark, Milton Keynes.
Riches, N. G. (2012). Sentence repetition in children with specific language impairment: an investigation of underlying mechanisms. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
47
(5), 499–510.
Roqueta, C. A., & Katsos, N. (2020). A distinction between linguistic and social pragmatics helps the precise characterization of pragmatic challenges in children with autism spectrum disorders and developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
63
1, 1494–1508.
Roselló, B., Berenguer, C., Navío, P., Baixauli, I., & Miranda, A. (2017). Executive Functioning, Social Cognition, Pragmatics, and Social Interaction in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Autism Spectrum Disorder. Current Developmental Disorders Reports,
4
1, 72–77.
Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). ADI-R: The Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Sandgren, O. & Holmström, K. (2015). Executive functions in mono- and bilingual children with language impairment – issues for speech-language pathology. Frontiers in Psychology,
6
1, 1074.
Schaeffer, J., Durrleman, S. & Eigsti, I. E. (2021). On links between language development and extralinguistic cognitive knowledge: What we can learn from autism, Language Acquisition,
28
:
1
, 1–5,
Schneider, P., Dubé, R. V., & Hayward, D. (2005). The Edmonton Narrative Norms Instrument. Retrieved from University of Alberta Faculty of Rehabilitation Medicine website: [URL]
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime User’s Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.
Schroeder, R. C. (2018). Do Bilinguals Have an Advantage in Theory of Mind? A Meta Analysis. Frontiers in Communication.
Senju, A. (2013). Atypical development of spontaneous social cognition in autism spectrum disorders. Brain & development,
35
(2), 96–101.
Sharaan, S., Fletcher-Watson, S., & MacPherson, S. E. (2021). The impact of bilingualism on the executive functions of autistic children: A study of English-Arabic children. Autism Research,
14
(3), 533–544.
Sheng, L., & McGregor, K. (2010). Lexical–Semantic Organization in Children With Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
53
(1), 146–159.
Sheng, L., Shi, H., Wang, D., Hao, Y., & Zheng, L. (2020). Narrative production in Mandarin-speaking children: Effects of language ability and elicitation method. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
63
(3), 774–792.
Siegel, D. J., Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (1996). Wechsler IQ profiles in diagnosis of high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
26
1, 389–406.
Silleresi, S., Tuller, L., Delage, H., Durrleman, S., Bonnet-Brilhault, F., Malvy, J., & Prevost, P. (2018). Sentence repetition and language impairment in French-speaking children with ASD. In A. Gavarró (Eds.), Language Acquisition and Language Disorders (pp. 235–258). John Benjamins, Amsterdam.
Smith, N., & Tsimpli, I. (1995). The mind of a savant: Language learning and modularity. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Stenbäck, V., Hällgren, M., & Larsby, B. (2016). Executive functions and working memory capacity in speech communication under adverse conditions. Speech, Language and Hearing,
19
(4), 218–226.
Stokes, S. F., Wong, A. M-Y., Fletcher, P., & Leonard, L. B. (2006). Nonword Repetition and Sentence Repetition as Clinical Markers of Specific Language Impairment: The Case of Cantonese. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools,
49
(2), 219–236.
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1999). A psychological approach to understanding the social and language impairments in autism. International Review of Psychiatry,
11
1, 325–334.
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2004). Identifying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism. In U. Frith & E. Hill (Eds.), Autism: Mind and brain (pp. 43–66). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Terzi, A., Marinis, T. & Francis, K. (2016). The Interface of Syntax with Pragmatics and Prosody in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
46
1, 2692–2706.
Thomaidis, L., Mavroeidi, N., Richardson, C., Choleva, A., Damianos, G., Bolias, K., & Tsolia, M. (2020). Autism Spectrum Disorders in Greece: Nationwide Prevalence in 10-11-Year-Old Children and Regional Disparities. Journal of Clinical Medicine,
9
(7), 2163.
Thordardottir, E. (2011). The relationship between bilingual exposure and vocabulary development. International Journal of Bilingualism,
15
(4), 426–445.
Tomasello, M. (1992). The social bases of language acquisition. Social Development,
1
(1), 67–87.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tsimpli, I. M. (2013). Autistic savants. In P. Robinson (Eds.), The Routledge encyclopaedia of second language acquisition (pp. 51–53). New York: Routledge.
Tsimpli, I. M., Peristeri, E., & Andreou, M. (2016). Narrative production in monolingual and bilingual children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics,
37
(1), 195–216.
Vogindroukas, I., Protopapas, A., & Sideridis, G. (2009). Experiment on the Expressive Vocabulary (Greek Version of Renfrew Word Finding Vocabulary Test). Chania: Glafki.
Vugs, B., Knoors, H., Cuperus, J., Hendriks, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2016). Executive function training in children with SLI: A pilot study. Child Language Teaching and Therapy,
33
(1), 1–20.
Wardlow, L. (2013). Individual differences in speakers’ perspective taking: The roles of executive control and working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,
20
1, 766–772.
Wechsler, D. (1991). The Wechsler intelligence scale for children – third edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Wermelinger, S., Gampe, A., & Daum, M. M. (2017). Bilingual toddlers have advanced abilities to repair communication failure. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
155
1, 84–94.
Williams, S., Moore, K., Crossman, A. M., & Talwar, V. (2016). The role of executive functions and theory of mind in children’s prosocial lie-telling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
141
1, 256–266.
Wing, L. (1997). The autistic spectrum. The Lancet,
350
1, 1761–1766.
Wynn, C. J., Borrie, S. A., & Sellers, T. P. (2018). Speech Rate Entrainment in Children and Adults With and Without Autism Spectrum Disorder. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology: A Journal of Clinical Practice,
27
(3), 965–974.
Yanjie, Y. X. S. (2001). Global precedence of hierarchical figure perception in children. Acta Psychologica Sinica,
33
(2), 142–147.
Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M. & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998) Meta-analyses comparing theory of mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin,
124
(3), 283–307.
Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445–469). Blackwell Publishing.
Cited by (6)
Cited by six other publications
Ferreira, Grace Joplin, Jay Getten & Henri Pesonen
2024. Strengths-Based Approaches to Autism Language and Communications Research: Where Are We?. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
Peristeri, Eleni, Margreet Vogelzang, Ianthi Maria Tsimpli & Stephanie Durrleman
2024. Bilingualism and second‐order theory of mind development in autistic children over time: Longitudinal relations with language, executive functions, and intelligence. Autism Research 17:9 ► pp. 1818 ff.
Carston, Robyn
2023. The relevance of words and the language/communication divide. Frontiers in Psychology 14
2023. Linguistic markers of autism spectrum conditions in narratives: A comprehensive analysis. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments 8
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 15 november 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.