Article published In:
Gesture
Vol. 15:2 (2016) ► pp.250282
References (200)
Ackermann, Hermann & Wolfram Ziegler (2010). Brain mechanisms underlying speech motor control. In William Hardcastle, John Laver, & Fiona Gibbon (Eds.), The handbook of phonetic sciences (2nd ed.) (pp. 202–250). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andrew, Richard (1963). Evolution of facial expression. Science, 142 (3595), 1034–1041. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arbib, Michael (2002). The mirror system, imitation, and the evolution of language. In Kerstin Dautenhahn & Chrystopher Nehaniv (Eds.), Imitation in animals and artifacts. Complex adaptive systems (pp. 229–280). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 281, 105–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006). The Mirror System Hypothesis on the linkage of action and languages. In Michael Arbib (Ed.), Action to language via mirror neuron system (pp. 3–47). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). How the brain got language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, David, William C. Stokoe, & Sherman E. Wilcox (1994). Signs of the origin of syntax. Current Anthropology, 35 (4), 349–368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995). Gesture and the nature of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, David & Sherman Wilcox (2007). The gestural origin of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ashcroft, Bill (2001). Language and race. Social Identities, 7 (3), 311–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
August, Peter & John Anderson (1987). Mammal sounds and motivation-structural rules: A test of the hypothesis. Journal of Mammology, 681, 1–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bard, Kim A. (2007). Neonatal imitation in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) tested with two paradigms. Animal Cognition, 1021, 233–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Harold R. (1987). Frequency code: Orofacial correlates of fundamental frequency. Phonetica, 441, 173–191. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, Janet B. & Nicole Chovil (1997). Faces in dialogue. In James A. Russell & José Miguel Fernández-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 334–346). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergman, Thore J. (2013). Speech-like vocalized lip-smacking in geladas. Current Biology, 231, R268–R269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Birdwhistell, Raymond L. (1970). Kinesics and context: Essays on body motion communication. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press,Google Scholar
Bolhuis, Johan J., Ian Tattersall, Noam Chomsky, & Robert C. Berwick (2014). How could language have evolved? PLOS Biology, 121, e1001934. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bosman, Conrado, Vladimir Lopes, & Francisco Aboitz (2005). Sharpening Occam’s razor: Is there need for a hand-signing stage prior to vocal communication? [Peer commentary on “From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics” by Michael A. Arbib]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28 (2), 105–167. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Browman, Catherine P. & Louis Goldstein (1989). Articulatory gestures as phonological units. Phonology, 61, 201–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brunner, Lawrence J. (1979). Smiles can be back channels. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37 (5), 728–734. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Buccino, Giovanni, Ferdinand Binkofski, & Lucia Riggio (2004). The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain and Language, 891, 370–376. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burling, Robbins (2005). The talking ape: How language evolved. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Call, Josep & Malinda Carpenter (2003). On imitation in apes and children. Infancia y aprendizaje, 261, 325–349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Capek, Cheryl M., Dafydd Waters, Bencie Woll, Mairéad MacSweeney, Michael J. Brammer, Philip K. McGuire, Anthony S. David, & Ruth Campbell (2008). Hand and mouth: Cortical correlates of lexical processing in British Sign Language and speechreading English. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 201, 1220–1234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chevalier-Skolnikoff, Suzanne (1973). Facial expression of emotion of nonhuman primates. In Paul Ekman (Ed.), Darwin and facial expression: A century of research in review (pp. 11–89). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chibelushi, Claude C., Farzin Deravi, & John S.D. Mason (2002). A review of speech-based bimodal recognition. IEEE Transactions on Multimedia, 41, 23–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chovil, Nicole (1991/1992). Discourse-oriented facial displays in conversation. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 251, 163–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997). Facing others: A social communicative perspective on facial displays. In James A. Russell & José Miguel Fernández-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 321–333). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. & Richard J. Gerrig (1990). Quotations as demonstrations. Language, 661, 764–805. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, Nathaniel & Marcus Perlman (2014). Breath, vocal and supralaryngeal flexibility in a human-reared gorilla. In Bart de Boer & Tessa Verhoef (Eds.), Proceedings of the Workshop on Evolution of Signals, Speech, and Signs (pp. 5–9). Vienna: Evolang Press.Google Scholar
Clay, Zanna & Klaus Zuberbühler (2014). Vocal communication and social awareness in chimpanzees and bonobos: insights from studies of vocal communication. In Daniel Dor, Chris Knight, & Jerome Lewis (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 141–156). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, Christopher (2013). Paleopoetics. The evolution of the literary imagination. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Corballis, Michael C. (2002). From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
. (2003). From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 261, 199–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2012). How language evolved from manual gestures. Gesture, 121, 200–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coudé, Gino, Pier Francesco Ferrari, Francesco Rodà, Monica Maranesi, Eleonora Borelli, Vania Veroni, Fabio Monti, Stefano Rozzi, & Leonardo Fogassi (2011). Neurons controlling voluntary vocalization in the macaque ventral premotor cortex. PLoS One, 61, e26822. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cowley, Stephen J. (2011). Distributed language. In Stephen J. Cowley (Ed.), Distributed language (pp. 1–14). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Darwin, Charles (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: Murray. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Donald, Merlin (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dor, Daniel, Chris Knight, & Jerome Lewis (2014). Introduction: A social perspective on how language began. In Daniel Dor, Chris Knight, & Jerome Lewis (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 1–14). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diamond, Arthur (1959). The history and origin of language. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Dunbar, Robin (1996). Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Eberl, Eva (2010). Control of gestures and vocalizations in primates. In Stefan Brudzynski (Ed.), Handbook of mammalian vocalization (pp. 469–476). Amsterdam: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eccardt, Thomas (2006). The case for articulatory gestures-not sounds-as the physical embodiment of speech signs. Studies in Functional and Structural Linguistics, 571, 283–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ekman, Paul (1979). About brows: Emotional and conversational signals. In Mario von Cranach, Klaus Foppa, Wolfg Lepenies, & Detlev Ploog (Eds.), Human ethology (pp. 169–202). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, Paul & Wallace V. Friesen (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 11, 49–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ekman, Paul, Wallace V. Friesen, & Joseph C. Hager (2002). Facial action coding system. The manual. Salt Lake City: Research Nexus.Google Scholar
Enard, Wolfgang, Molly Przeworski, Simon E. Fisher, Cecilia S.L. Lai, Victor Wiebe, Takashi Kitano, Anthony P. Monaco, & Svante Pääbo (2002). Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature, 4181, 869–872. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fedurek, Pawel, Katie E. Slocombe, Jessica A. Hartel, & Klaus Zuberbühler (2015). Chimpanzee lip-smacking facilitates cooperative behaviour. Scientific Reports, 5, 13460. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferrari, Pier F., Vittorio Gallese, Giacomo Rizzolatti, & Leonardo Fogassi (2003). Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience, 171, 1703–1714. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh (2010). The evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fitch, W. Tecumseh, Mark D. Hauser, & Noam Chomsky (2005). The evolution of the language faculty: Clarifications and implications. Cognition, 971, 179–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forrester, Gillian & Alina Rodriguez (2015). Slip of the tongue: Implications for evolution and language development. Cognition, 1411, 103–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fridlund, Alan J. (1997). The new ethology of facial expressions. In James A. Russell & José Miguel Fernández-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 103–129). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Galantucci, Bruno, Carol A. Fowler, & M.T. Turvey (2006). The motor theory of speech perception reviewed. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13 (3), 361–377. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gardner, R. Allen & Beatrice T. Gardner (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165 (3894), 664–672. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gazzola, Valeria, Lisa Aziz-Zadeh, & Christian Keysers (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Current Biology, 161, 1824–1829. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentilucci, Maurizio & Michael C. Corballis (2006). From manual gesture to speech: A gradual transition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 301, 949–960. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentilucci, Maurizio & Riccardo Dalla Volta (2008). Spoken language and arm gestures are controlled by the same motor control system. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 611, 944–957. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ghazanfar, Asif A. & Nikos K. Logothetis (2003). Neuroperception: Facial expressions linked to monkey calls. Nature, 4231, 937–938. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ghazanfar, Asif & David Lewkowicz (2008). The ontogeny and phylogeny of bimodal primate vocal communication. In Nobuo Masataka (Ed.), The origins of language (pp. 85–110). Tokyo: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ghazanfar, Asif A., Daniel Y. Takahashi, Neil Mathur, & Tecumseh Fitch (2012). Cineradiography of monkey lip-smacking reveals putative precursors of speech dynamics. Current Biology, 221, 1176–1182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gillespie-Lynch, Kristen, Patricia M. Greenfield, Heidi Lyn, & Sue Savage-Rumbaugh (2014). Gestural and symbolic development among apes and humans: Support for a multimodal theory of language evolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goffman, Erving (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Garden City: Doubleday Anchor Books.Google Scholar
(1963). Behavior in public places: Notes on the social organization of gatherings. New York & London: The Free Press of Glencoe.Google Scholar
(1969). Strategic interaction. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Gogate, Lakshmi J. & Lorraine E. Bahrick (1998). Intersensory redundancy facilitates learning of arbitrary relations between vowel sounds and objects in seven-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 691, 133–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan (2014). Widening the lens: What the manual modality reveals about language, learning and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 3691, 20130295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan & Melissa A. Singer (2003). From children’s hands to adults’ ears: Gesture’s role in teaching and learning. Developmental Psychology, 39 (3), 509–520. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, Susan, Wing Chee So, Aslı Özyürek, & Carolyn Mylander (2008). The natural order of events: How speakers of different languages represent events nonverbally. PNAS, 105 (27), 9163–9168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodall, Jane (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Publishing.Google Scholar
Grice, H. Paul (1975). Logic and conversation. In Peter Cole & Jerry L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics, Vol. 3: Speech acts (pp. 43–58). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, Michael A.K. (1973). Explorations in the functions of language. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Harpham, Geoffrey G. (2009). Roots, races, and the return to philology. Representations, 106 (1), 34–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, Marc D. (1993). The evolution of nonhuman primate vocalizations: Effects of phylogeny, body weight and motivational state. American Naturalist, 1421, 528–542. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1997). The evolution of communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, Mark D., Noam Chomsky, & W. Tecumseh Fitch (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 2981, 1569–1579. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawhee, Debra (2006). Language as sensuous action: Sir Richard Paget, Kenneth Burke, and gesture-speech theory. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 921, 331–354. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hewes, Gordon W. (1973). Primate communication and the gestural origin of language. Current Anthropology, 141, 5–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1975). Language origins: A bibliography. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
. (1976). The current status of the gestural theory of language origin. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 2801, 482–504. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1977a). A model for language evolution. Sign Language Studies, 151, 97–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1977b). Language origin theories. In Duane Rumbaugh (Ed.), Language learning by a chimpanzee: The Lana Project (pp. 3–53). New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1996). A history of the study of language origins and the gestural primacy hypothesis. In Andy Lock & Charles Peters (Eds.), Handbook of human symbolic evolution (pp. 263–269). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Higginbotham, David R., Matthew I. Isaak, & James N. Domingue (2008). The exaptation of manual dexterity for articulate speech: An electromyogram investigation. Experimental Brain Research, 1861, 603–609. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hinde, Robert & Thelma Rowell (1962). Communication by postures and facial expressions in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London, 138 (1), 1–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hobaiter, Catherine & W. Byrne Richard (2012). Gesture use in consortship: Wild chimpanzees’ use of gesture for an ‘evolutionarily urgent’ purpose. In Simone Pika & Katja Liebal (Eds.), Developments in primate gesture research (pp. 127–144). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hockett, Charles. F. (1958). A course in modern linguistics. New York: Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1959). Animal “languages” and human language. Human Biology, 311, 32–39.Google Scholar
Hockett, Charles F. (1960a). The origin of speech. Scientific American, 2031, 88–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1960b). Logical considerations in the study of animal communication. In Wesley Lanyon & William Tavolga (Eds.), Animal sounds and communication (pp. 392–430). Washington: American Institute of Biological Sciences.Google Scholar
Hockett, Charles. F. (1966). The problem of universals in language. In Jospeh Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of language (pp. 1–29). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hockett, Charles F. & Stuart A. Altmann (1968). A note on design features. In Thomas A. Sebeok (Ed.), Animal communication: Techniques of study and results of research (pp. 61–72). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Hooff, Jan van (1962). Facial expressions in higher primates. Symposia of the Zoological Society of London, 81, 97–125.Google Scholar
(1967). The facial displays of the catarrhine monkeys and apes. In Desmond Morris (Ed.), Primate ethology (pp. 7–68). Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
(1972). A comparative approach to the phylogeny of laughter and smiling. In Robert Hinde (Ed.), Non-verbal communication (pp. 209–238). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hurford, James R. (2007). The origins of meaning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Imai, Mitsumi & Sotaro Kita (2014). The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 20130298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, Jintao, Abeer Alwan, Patricia A. Keating, Edward T. Auer Jr., & Lynne E. Bernstein (2002). On the relationship between face movements, tongue movements, and speech acoustics. EURASIP Journal on Applied Signal Processing, 111, 1174–1188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jóhanneson, Alexander (1949). Origin of language. Four essays. Reykjavik: Leiftur.Google Scholar
Johansson, Sverker (2005). Origins of language. Constraints and hypotheses. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kegl, Judy, Ann Senghas, & Marie Coppola (1999). Creation through contact: Sign language emergence and sign language change in Nicaragua. In Michel DeGraff (Ed.), Language creation and language change: Creolization, diachrony, and development (pp. 179–237). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kendon, Adam (1972). Some relationships between body motion and speech. An analysis of an example. In Aron Wolfe Siegman & Benjamin Pope (Eds.), Studies in dyadic communication (pp. 177–210). New York: Pergamon Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1991). Some considerations for a theory of language origins. Man, 261, 199–221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004). Gesture. Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008). Signs for language origins? The Public Journal of Semiotics, 21, 2–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011). ‘Gesture first’ and ‘speech first’ in theories of language origins. In Gaurav Mathur & Donna Jo Napoli (Eds.), Deaf around the world (pp. 251–267). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2014a). The ‘poly-modalic’ nature of utterances and its relevance for inquiring into language origins. In Daniel Dor, Chris Knight, & Jerome Lewis (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 67–76). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kendon (2014b) Semiotic diversity in utterance production and the concept of ‘language’. Philosophical Transaction of the Royal Society, Series B, 3691: 20130293. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kohler, Evelyne, Christian Keysers, M. Alessandra Umilta, Leonrado Fogassi, Vittorio Gallese, & Giacomo Rizzolatti (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 2971, 846–848. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, George & Mark Johnson (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(1999). Philosophy in the flesh. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar, Vol. I: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Leavens, David A., Jamie L. Russell, & William D. Hopkins (2010). Multimodal communication by captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) . Animal Cognition, 131, 33–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leavens, David, Jared Taglialatela, & William Hopkins (2014). From grasping to grooming to gossip: innovative use of chimpanzee signals in novel environments supports both vocal and gestural theories of language origins. In Marco Pina & Nathalie Gontier (Eds.), The evolution of social communication in primates: A multidisciplinary approach (pp. 179–194). New York: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Le Cabec, Adeline, Philipp Gunz, Kornelius Kupczik, José Braga, & Jean-Jacques Hublin (2013). Anterior tooth root morphology and size in Neanderthals: Taxonomic and functional implications. Journal of Human Evolution, 641, 169–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, Steven C. & Judith Holler (2014). The origin of human multi-modal communication. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369, 201303302. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewis, Jerome (2014). BaYaka Pygmy multi-modal and mimetic communication traditions. In Daniel Dor, Chris Knight, & Jerome Lewis (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 77–91). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liberman, Alvin, Franklin Cooper, Donald Shankweiler, & Michael Studdert-Kennedy, (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 741, 431–461. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liberman, Alvin M. & Ignatius G. Mattingly (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revisited. Cognition, 211, 1–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liberman, Alvin M. & Doug H. Whalen (2000). On the relation of speech to language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4 (5), 187–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liebal, Katja, Bridget M. Waller, Anne M. Burrows, & Katie E. Slocombe (2014). Primate communication: A multimodal approach. Cambridge & New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Linell, Per (2005). The written language bias in linguistics: Its nature, origins, and transformations. London: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, Ulf (2014). Two sources of meaning in infant communication: Preceding action contexts and act-accompanying characteristics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369, 20130294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Little, Anthony C., Benedict C. Jones, Corri Waitt, Bernard P. Tiddeman, David R. Feinberg, David I. Perrett, Coren L. Apicella, & Frank W. Marlowe (2008). Symmetry is related to sexual dimorphism in faces: data across culture and species. PLoS ONE, 31, e2106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Little, Anthony C., Benedict C. Jones, & Lisa M. DeBruine (2011). Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary based research. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 3661, 1638–1659. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacNeilage, Peter F. (2008). The origin of speech. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, Peter F. & Barbara L. Davies (2005). Evolutionary sleight of hand: Then, they saw it; now we don’t. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 281, 153–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto-Oda, Akiko & Masaki Tomonaga (2005). “Intentional” control of sound production found in leaf-clipping display of Mahale chimpanzees. Journal of Ethology, 231, 109–112. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNeill, David (1992). Hand and mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(2000). Growth points in thinking-for-speaking. In David McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 141–161). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). How language began. Gesture and speech in human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(in press). Why we gesture: The surprising role of the hands in communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
McNeill, David, Susan D. Duncan, Jonathan Cole, Shaun Gallagher, & Bennett Bertenthal (2008). Growth points from the very beginning. Interaction Studies, 91, 117–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marcus, Garry F. & Simon E. Fisher (2003). FOXP2 in focus: what can genes tell us about speech and language? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 71, 257–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maynard Smith, John & David Harper (2003). Animal signals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McGurk, Harry & John MacDonald (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 2641, 746–748. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meguerditchian, Adrien, Hélène Cochet, & Jacques Vauclair (2011). From gesture to language: ontogenetic and phylogenetic perspectives on gestural communication and its cerebral lateralization. In Anne Vilain, Jean-Luc Schwartz, Christian Abry, & Jacques Vauclair (Eds.), Primate communication and human language: vocalization, gestures, imitation and deixis in humans and non-humans (pp. 89–118). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meguerditchian, Adrien, Marie Plouvier, Jill Pruetz, & William Hopkins (2014). From hand to mouth: fine precision grip during mutual grooming elicited wide lip movements in wild fongoli chimpanzees. In Erica Cartmill, Sean Roberts, Heidi Lyn, & Hannah Cornish (Eds.), The evolution of language. Proceedings of the 10th International Conference (EVOLANG 10) (pp. 487–488). Singapore: World Scientific.Google Scholar
Meguerditchian, Adrien & Jacques Vauclair (2014). Communicative signaling, lateralization and brain substrate in nonhuman primates: Toward a gestural or a multimodal origin of language? Humana Mente . Journal of Philosophical Studies, 271, 135–160.Google Scholar
Morton, Eugene S. (1977). On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. American Naturalist, 1111, 855–869. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mühlenbernd, Roland, Dankmar Enke, Matthias Villing, Natalja Gavrilov, & Jonas David Nick (2014). Modality switch in human language evolution. In Erica A. Cartmill, Seán Roberts, Heidi Lyn, & Hannah Cornish (Eds.), The evolution of language. Proceedings of the Tenth International Conference EVOLANG (pp. 161–168). Singapore: World Scientific.Google Scholar
Ohala, John (1983). Cross-language use of pitch: an ethological view. Phonetica, 40 (1), 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994). The frequency code underlies the sound-symbolic use of voice-pitch. In Leanne Hinton, Johann Nichols, & John Ohala (Eds.), Sound symbolism (pp. 325–347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Orzechowski, Sylwester, Sławomir Wacewicz, & Przemysław Żywiczyński (2014). Orofacial gestures in language evolution. The auditory feedback hypothesis. In Erica Cartmill, Sean Roberts, Heidi Lyn, & Hannah Cornish (Eds.), The evolution of language. Proceedings of the 10th International Conference (EVOLANG 10) (pp. 221–227). Singapore: World Scientific.Google Scholar
Özyürek, Aslı (2014). Hearing and seeing meaning in speech and gesture: Insights from brain and behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369 (1651), 20130296. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paget, Richard A.S. (1930). Human speech: Some observations, experiments, and conclusions as to the nature, origin, purpose and possible improvement of human speech. London: Kegan Paul & Trench Trübner.Google Scholar
. (1944). The origin of language. Science, 991, 14–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parr, Lisa A. (2004). Perceptual biases for multimodal cues in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) affect recognition. Animal Cognition, 71, 171–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parr, Lisa A. & Frans B.M. de Waal (1999). Visual kin recognition in chimpanzees. Nature, 3991, 647–648. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parr, Lisa A. & Bridget M. Waller (2006). Understanding chimpanzee facial expression: Insights into the evolution of communication. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 221–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parr, Lisa A., Matthew Heintz, Elizabeth Lonsdorf, & Emily Wroblewski (2010). Visual kin recognition in nonhuman primates (Pan troglodytes and Macaca mulatta): Inbreeding avoidance or male distinctiveness? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1241, 343–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Petrides, Michael, Geneviève Cadoret, & Scott Mackey (2005). Orofacial somatomotor responses in the macaque monkey homologue of Broca’s area. Nature, 4351, 1235–1238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Petrides, Michael & Deepak N. Pandya (2009). Distinct parietal and temporal pathways to the homologues of Broca’s area in the monkey. PLoS Biol, 71, e1000170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Steven (1994). The language instinct. New York: Harper Perennial Modern Classics. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pyers, Jennie E. & Karen Emmorey (2008). The face of bimodal bilingualism: Grammatical markers in American Sign Language are produced when bilinguals speak to English monolinguals. Psychological Science, 191, 531–535. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ravizza, Susan (2005). Neural regions associated with categorical speech perception and production. In Henri Cohen & Claire Lefebvre (Eds.), Handbook of categorization in cognitive science (pp. 601–615). Amsterdam: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ricci Bitti, Pio & Isabella Poggi (1991). Symbolic nonverbal behavior: Talking through gestures. In Robert Feldman & Bernard Rime (Eds.), Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior (pp. 433–457). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Roll, Patrice, Gabrielle Rudolf, Sandrine Pereira, Barbara Royer, Ingrid E. Scheffer, Annick Massacrier, Maria-Paola Valenti, Nathalie Roeckel-Trevisiol, Sarah Jamali, Christophe Beclin, Caroline Seegmuller, Marie-Noëlle Metz-Lutz, Arnaud Lemainque, Marc Delepine, Christophe Caloustian, Anne de Saint Martin, Nadine Bruneau, Danièle Depétris, Marie-Geneviève Mattéi, Elisabeth Flori, Andrée Robaglia-Schlupp, Nicolas Lévy, Bernd A. Neubauer, Rivka Ravid, Christian Marescaux, Samuel F. Berkovic, Edouard Hirsch, Mark Lathrop, Pierre Cau, & Pierre Szepetowski (2006). SRPX2 mutations in disorders of language cortex and cognition. Human Molecular Genetics, 151, 1195–1207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sandler, Wendy (2009). Symbiotic symbolization by hand and mouth in sign language. Semiotica, 2009 (174), 241–275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013). Vive la différence: Sign language and spoken language in language evolution. Language and Cognition, 51, 189–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sereno, Martin (2014). Origin of symbol-using systems: Speech, but not sign, without the semantic urge. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 3691, 20130303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sia, Gek-Ming, Roger Clem, & Richard Huganir (2013). The human language-associated gene SRPX2 regulates synapse formation and vocalization in mice. Science, 342 (6161), 987–991. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skipper, Jeremy (2014). Echoes of the spoken past: How auditory cortex hears context during speech perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369, 20130297. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, Katie E., Bridget M. Waller, & Katja Liebal (2011). The language void: The need for multimodality in primate communication research. Animal Behaviour, 811, 919–924. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, Dan & Deirdre Wilson (1986) Relevance: Communication and cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Stam, James H. (1976). Inquiries into the origin of language: The fate of a question. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Steklis, Horst & Stevan R. Harnad (1976). From hand to mouth: Some critical stages in the evolution of language. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 2801, 445–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, Michael (2002). Mirror neurons, vocal imitation and the evolution of particulate principle. In Maxim Stamenov & Vittorio Gallese (Eds.), Mirror neurons and the evolution of brain and language (pp. 207–227). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sueyoshi, Ayano & Debra M. Hardison (2005). The role of gestures and facial cues in second language listening comprehension. Language Learning, 551, 661–699. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Summerfield, Quentin (1992). Lipreading and audio-visual speech perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 3351, 71–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sutton, Dwight (1979). Mechanisms underlying vocal control in nonhuman primates. In Horst Steklis & Michael Raleigh (Eds.), Neurobiology of social communication in primates: An evolutionary perspective (pp. 45–68). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sutton-Spence, Rachel (2005). Analysing sign language poetry. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Swadesh, Morris (1971). The origin and diversification of language. Chicago: Alidne & Atherton.Google Scholar
Sweet, Henry (1888). A history of English sounds from the earliest period, with full word-lists. Oxford: Clarendon (retrieved from [URL]).Google Scholar
Taglialatela, Jared P., Jamie L. Russell, Jennifer A. Schaeffer, & William D. Hopkins (2011). Chimpanzee vocal signaling points to a multimodal origin of human language. PLoS ONE, 6 (4), e18852. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taglialatela, Jared P., Jamie L. Russell, Sarah M. Pope, Tamara Morton, Stephanie Bogart, Lisa A. Reamer, Steven J. Schapiro, & William D. Hopkins (2015). Multimodal communication in chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 77 (11), 1143–1148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tallerman, Maggie (2012). Protolanguage. In Maggie Tallerman & Kathleen Gibson (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution (pp. 479–491). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tallerman, Maggie, & Kathleen Gibson (Eds.) (2012). The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vainio, Lari, Mikko Tiainen, Kaisa Tiippana, & Martti Vainio (2014). Shared processing of planning articulatory gestures and grasping. Experimental Brain Research, 232 (7), 2359–2368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vargha-Khadem, Faraneh, Kate E. Watkins, C.J. Price, John Ashburner, Katie J. Alcock, Alan Connelly, Richard S.J. Frackowiak, Karl J. Friston, M.E. Pembrey, Mortimer Mishkin, David G. Gadian, & Richard E. Passingham (1998). Neural basis of an inherited speech and language disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95 (21), 12695–12700. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vigliocco, Gabriella, Pamela Perniss, & David Vinson (2014). Introduction: Language as a multimodal phenomenon: Implications for language learning, processing and evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 3691, 20130292. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, Lev S. (1978). Mind in society. The development of higher psychological processes. Michael Cole, Vera John-Steiner, Sylvia Scribner, & Ellen Souberman (Eds.). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
. (1987). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, Vol. 1: Problems of general psychology. Robert W. Rieber & Aaron S. Carton (Eds.). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Wacewicz, Sławomir & Przemysław Żywiczyński (2015a). Language evolution: Why Hockett’s design features are a non-starter. Biosemiotics, 8 (1), 29–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015b). From the narrow to the broad. Multiple perspectives on language evolution. Theoria et Historia Scientiarum, 111, 5–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wallace, Alfred R. (1881). Review of Tylor’s Anthropology . Nature, 241, 242–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (1895). The expressiveness of speech, or mouth gesture as a factor in the evolution of speech. Fortnightly Review, 581, 528–543.Google Scholar
Waters, Gabriel S. & Roger S. Fouts (2002). Sympathetic mouth movements accompanying fine motor movements in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) with implications toward the evolution of language. Neurological Research, 241, 174–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Watkins, Kate & Tomáš Paus (2004). Modulation of motor excitability during speech perception: the role of Broca’s area. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 161, 978–987. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Watson, Stuart K., Simon W. Townsend, Anne M. Schel, Claudia Wilke, Emma K. Wallace, Leveda Cheng, Victoria West, & Katie Slocombe (2015). Vocal learning in the functionally referential food grunts of chimpanzees. Current Biology, 251, 495–499. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Watts, Richard (2003). Politeness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Woll, Bencie (2001). The sign that dares to speak its name: Echo phonology in British Sign Language (BSL). In Penny Boyes-Braem & Rachel Sutton-Spence (Eds.), The hands are the head of the mouth (pp. 87–98). Hamburg: Signum Press.Google Scholar
(2014). Moving from hand to mouth: echo phonology and the origins of language. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 662. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yehia, Hani, Philip Rubin, & Eric Vatikiotis-Bateson (1998). Quantitative association of vocal-tract and facial behavior. Speech Communication, 261, 23–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yehia, Hani C., Takaaki Kuratate, & Eric Vatikiotis-Bateson (2002). Linking facial animation, head motion and speech acoustics. Journal of Phonetics, 301, 555–568. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zimmermann, Elke, Lisette Leliveld, & Simone Schehka (2013). Toward the evolutionary roots of affective prosody in human acoustic communication: A comparative approach to mammalian voices. In Eckart Altenmüller, Sabine Schmidt, & Elke Zimmermann (Eds.), Evolution of emotional communication: From sounds in nonhuman mammals to speech and music in man (pp. 116–132). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zlatev, Jordan (2014). Human uniqueness, bodily mimesis and the evolution of language. Humana Mente. Journal of Philosophical Studies, 271, 197–219.Google Scholar
Cited by (11)

Cited by 11 other publications

Żywiczyński, Przemysław & Sławomir Wacewicz
2022. From Body to Language: Gestural and Pantomimic Scenarios of Language Origin in the Enlightenment. Topoi 41:3  pp. 539 ff. DOI logo
Mortazavi, Mohsen & Afshin Davarpanah
2021. Implementation of a Thematic Analysis Method to Develop a Qualitative Model on the Authentic Foreign Language Learning Perspective: A Case Study in the University of Northern Cyprus. Education Sciences 11:9  pp. 544 ff. DOI logo
Vainio, Lari
2019. Connection between movements of mouth and hand: Perspectives on development and evolution of speech. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 100  pp. 211 ff. DOI logo
Liebal, Katja
2018. Primate Communication. In Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior,  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Liebal, Katja
2019. Sprachevolution und Emotion. Ein artvergleichender Ansatz. In Emotionen,  pp. 253 ff. DOI logo
Liebal, Katja
2022. Primate Communication. In Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior,  pp. 5539 ff. DOI logo
Dediu, Dan, Rick Janssen & Scott R. Moisik
2017. Language is not isolated from its wider environment: Vocal tract influences on the evolution of speech and language. Language & Communication 54  pp. 9 ff. DOI logo
Wacewicz, Sławomir & Przemyslaw Zywiczynski
2017. The multimodal origins of linguistic communication. Language & Communication 54  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Wacewicz, Sławomir, Przemysław Żywiczyński & Alessandra Chiera
2017. An evolutionary approach to low-level conversational cooperation. Language Sciences 63  pp. 91 ff. DOI logo
Zlatev, Jordan, Sławomir Wacewicz, Przemyslaw Zywiczynski & Joost van de Weijer
2017. Multimodal-first or pantomime-first?. Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems 18:3  pp. 465 ff. DOI logo
Zywiczynski, Przemyslaw, Nathalie Gontier & Slawomir Wacewicz
2017. The evolution of (proto-)language: Focus on mechanisms. Language Sciences 63  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 17 november 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.