Part of
Conceptualizations of Time
Edited by Barbara Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk
[Human Cognitive Processing 52] 2016
► pp. 6784
References (28)
References
Alverson, H. 1994. Semantics and Experience: Universal Metaphors of Time in English, Mandarin, Hindi, and Sesotho. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L. 2001. Does Language Shape Thought? Mandarin and English Speakers’ Conceptions of Time. Cognitive Psychology 43, 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D. 2008. Who’s Afraid of the Big Bad Whorf? Cross-linguistic Differences in Temporal Language and Thought. Language Learning 58(177), 63–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2009a. Embodiment of Abstract Concepts: Good and Bad in Right- and Left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 138(3), 351–367. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2009b. When is a Linguistic Metaphor a Conceptual Metaphor? In V. Evans & S. Pourcel (eds.), New Directions in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 127–145). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2011. Different Bodies, Different Minds: The Body-specificity of Language and Thought. Current Directions in Psychological Science 20(6), 378–383. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Bottini, R. 2014. Mirror reading can reverse the flow of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143(2), 473–479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Henetz, T. 2012. Handedness Shapes Children’s Abstract Concepts. Cognitive Science, 36, 359–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Jasmin, K. 2010. Good and Bad in the Hands of Politicians: Spontaneous Gestures during Positive and Negative Speech. PLoS ONE 5(7), e11805. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cienki, A. 1998. Metaphoric Gestures and Some of Their Relations to Verbal Metaphorical Expressions. In J.-P. Koenig (ed.), Discourse and Cognition: Bridging the Gap, 189–204.Google Scholar
Clark, H.H. 1973. Space, Time, Semantics, and the Child. In T.E. Moore (ed.), Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cooperrider, K., & Núñez, R. 2009. Across Time, Across the Body: Transversal Temporal Gestures. Gesture 9(2), 181–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de la Fuente, J. et al. 2014. When you Think about It, the Past is in front of You: How Culture Shapes Spatial Conceptions of Time. Psychological Science 25(9), 1682–1690. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, K. 2001. Space on Hand: The Exploitation of Signing Space to Illustrate Abstract Thought. In M. Gattis (ed.), Spatial Schemas and Abstract Thought (pp. 147–174). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Evans, V. 2004. The Structure of Time: Language, Meaning and Temporal Cognition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuhrman, O., & Boroditsky, L. 2010. Cross-cultural Differences in Mental Representations of Time: Evidence from an Implicit Nonlinguistic Task. Cognitive Science 34, 1430–1451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. 1980. Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mateo, J. 2010. Salud y ritual en Marruecos. Concepciones del cuerpo y prácticas de curación. [Health and Ritual in Morocco. Conceptions of the Body and Healing Practices.] Barcelona: Bellaterra.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. 1992. Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Moore, K. 2006. Space-to-time Mappings and Temporal Concepts. Cognitive Linguistics 17, 199–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R., & Sweetser, E. 2006. With the Future Behind Them: Convergent Evidence from Aymara Language and Gesture in the Crosslinguistic Comparison of Spatial Construals of Time. Cognitive Science 30, 401–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Parrill, F., & Sweetser, E. 2004. What we Mean by Meaning: Conceptual Integration in Gesture Analysis and Transcription. Gesture 4, 197–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ouellet, M. et al. 2010. Is the Future the Right Time? Experimental Psychology 57(4), 308–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Radden, G. 2004. The Metaphor TIME AS SPACE Across Languages. In N. Baumgarten, et al. (eds.), Übersetzen, interkulturelle Kommunikation, Spracherwerb und Sprachvermittlung – Das Leben mit Mehreren Sprachen: Festschrift für Juliane House zum 60 Geburtstag. Aks-verlag: Bochum, Germany.Google Scholar
Scott, A. 1989. The Vertical Dimension and Time in Mandarin. Australian Journal of Linguistics 9, 295–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Traugott, E. 1978. On the Expression of Spatio-temporal Relations in Language. In J.H. Greenberg (ed.), Universals of Human Language: Vol. 3. Word Structure (pp. 369–400). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Tversky, B. et al. 1991. Cross-cultural and Developmental Trends in Graphic Productions. Cognitive Psychology 23, 515–557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weger, U.W., & Pratt, J. 2008. Time Flies like an Arrow: Space–time Compatibility Effects Suggest the Use of a Mental Timeline. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 15(2), 426–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (9)

Cited by nine other publications

Kasia M. Jaszczolt
2023. Understanding Human Time, DOI logo
Gu, Yan
2022. Chapter 10. Time in Chinese hands. In Time Representations in the Perspective of Human Creativity [Human Cognitive Processing, 75],  pp. 209 ff. DOI logo
Piata, Anna, Adriana Gordejuela & Daniel Alcaraz Carrión
2022. Introduction. In Time Representations in the Perspective of Human Creativity [Human Cognitive Processing, 75],  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Sun, Ying, Yan Zhang, Ying Fang & Wenxing Yang
2022. Representations of Diagonal Timelines in English and Mandarin Speakers. European Journal of Psychology Open 81:3  pp. 89 ff. DOI logo
Yang, Wenxing, Jiaqi Dong, Ruidan Bi, Jian Gu & Xueqin Feng
2022. Cross-cultural differences in mental representations of diagonal time lines. Review of Cognitive Linguistics 20:2  pp. 357 ff. DOI logo
Valenzuela, Javier & Daniel Alcaraz Carrión
2020. Temporal Expressions in English and Spanish: Influence of Typology and Metaphorical Construal. Frontiers in Psychology 11 DOI logo
Gu, Yan, Yeqiu Zheng & Marc Swerts
2019. Which Is in Front of Chinese People, Past or Future? The Effect of Language and Culture on Temporal Gestures and Spatial Conceptions of Time. Cognitive Science 43:12 DOI logo
LI, Heng & Yu CAO
2018. 时间焦点对前后方向上内隐时空映射的影响——来自汉族和羌族的证据. Acta Psychologica Sinica 50:10  pp. 1083 ff. DOI logo
Li, Heng & Yu Cao
2018. Karma or Immortality: Can Religion Influence Space‐Time Mappings?. Cognitive Science 42:3  pp. 1041 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 17 november 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.