Mark Turner |
Case Western Reserve University, USA
In this chapter, we seek to show that the human mind can create blended discourse, or fictive communication, because it is able to do advanced conceptual blending. Thanks to advanced blending, human beings can integrate unrelated experiences and concepts into new mental wholes with novel properties. We analyze how instances of fictive communication are made possible by generic templates for conceptual blending. Fictive communication is a blending pattern combining several generic templates, most of which were not originated in relation to discourse. Fictive communication inherits the whole structure of fictive interaction, which involves fictivity, compression patterns, and an interaction frame that includes counterfactuality. Complex, classic blended joint attention is added, and the interaction frame selected is the one for communication.
(2001) Semantic leaps: Frame-shifting and conceptual blending in meaning construction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Coulson, S., & Pagán Cánovas, C.
(2013) Understanding timelines. Journal of Cognitive Semiotics, 5(1–2), 198–219.
Fauconnier, G.
(2005) Compression and emergent structure. Language and Linguistics, 6(4), 523–538.
Fauconnier, G.
(2009) Generalized integration networks. In V. Evans & S. Purcell (Eds.), New directions in cognitive linguistics (pp. 147–160). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Fauconnier, G.
(2010) Causal compressions in language and thought. In G. Fauconnier (Ed.), Ten lectures on cognitive construction of meaning. Beijing: FLTR Press.
Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M.
(1994) Conceptual projection and middle spaces (UCSD Department of Cognitive Science Technical Report No. 9401). San Diego.
(2012) The evolution of primate societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Mithen, S.
(1996) The origin of art: Natural signs, mental modularity, and visual symbolism. In E. Maschner (Ed.), Darwinian archaeologies (pp. 197–217). New York: Plenum.
Pagán Cánovas, C.
(2010) Erotic emissions in Greek poetry: A generic integration network. Cognitive Semiotics, 6, 7–32.
Pascual, E.
(2006) Fictive interaction within the sentence: A communicative type of fictivity in grammar. Cognitive Linguistics, 17(2), 245–267.
2020. Learning formulaic creativity: Chunking in verbal art and speech. Cognitive Semiotics 13:1
Piata, Anna & Cristóbal Pagán Cánovas
2017. Powerful rhyme and sluttish time: A cross-linguistic study of time personification in poetic discourse. Language and Literature: International Journal of Stylistics 26:1 ► pp. 18 ff.
TURNER, MARK
2017. Conceptual compression and alliterative form – a response to Harbus. English Language and Linguistics 21:2 ► pp. 221 ff.
Turner, Mark
2019. Compression and Decompression in Mathematics1. In Interdisciplinary Perspectives on Math Cognition [Mathematics in Mind, ], ► pp. 29 ff.
Vouvaris, Petros & Dimitris Tasoudis
2018. B(l)ending time, (de)compressing identity: Creative thought and meaning construction in Copy Shop (2001). Musicae Scientiae 22:1 ► pp. 88 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 17 november 2023. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.