References
Ausín, A. & Depiante, M
2000On the syntax of parecer with and without an experiencer. In Hispanic Linguistics at the Turn of the Millennium, H. Campos, A. Herburger, A. Morales-Front & T.J. Walsh (eds), 155-170. Sommerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Bardel, C. & Falk, Y
2007The role of the second language in third language acquisition: The case of Germanic syntax. Second Language Research 23: 459−484. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2012Behind the L2 status factor: A neurolinguistic framework for L3 research. In Cabrelli Amaro, et al. (eds), 61-78.Google Scholar
Berkes, É. & Flynn, S
2012Further evidence in support of the Cumulative-Enhancement Model: CP structure development. In Cabrelli Amaro, et al. (eds), 143-164.Google Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R
1989The logical problem of foreign language learning. Linguistic Analysis 20(1-2): 3-49.Google Scholar
2009The evolving context of the Fundamental Difference Hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 31: 175-198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borg, K
2013The acquisition of future of probability in L3 Spanish. Paper presented at the 12th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition, Gainesville, Florida.
Cabrelli Amaro, J., Flynn, S., & Rothman, J
(eds) 2012Third Language Acquisition in Adulthood. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Campos Dintrans, G., Pires, A., & Rothman, J
2014Subject-to-subject raising and the syntax of tense in L2 Spanish: A Full Access approach. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 17(1): 38-55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cenoz, J
2001The effect of linguistic distance, L2 status, and age on cross-linguistic influence in third language acquisition. In Cross-linguistic Influence in Third Language Acquisition, J. Cenoz, B. Hufeisen & U. Jessner (eds), 8-20. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Colman, A., Morris, C. & Preson, C
1997Comparing rating scales of different lengths: Equivalence of scores from 5-point and 7-point scales. Psychological Reports 80: 355-362. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dewaele, J.M
1998Lexical inventions: French interlanguage as L2 versus L3. Applied Linguistics 19: 471-490. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ecke, P. & Hall, C
2013Tracking tip-of-the-tongue states in a multilingual speaker: Evidence of attrition or instability in lexical systems? International Journal of Bilingualism 17(6): 734-751. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Falk, Y., Lindqvist, C. & Bardel, C
2014The role of L1 explicit metalinguistic knowledge in L3 oral production at the initial state. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Falk, Y., & Bardel, C
2011Object pronouns in German L3 syntax: Evidence for the L2 status factor. Second Language Research 27: 59-82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flynn, S., Foley, C. & Vinnitskaya, I
2004The Cumulative-Enhancement Model for language acquisition: Comparing adults’ and children’s patterns of development in first, second and third language acquisition of relative clauses. The International Journal of Multilingualism 1: 3-16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foote, R
2009Transfer and L3 acquisition: The role of typology. In Leung (ed.), 89-114.Google Scholar
García-Mayo, M.P. & Rothman, J
2012L3 morphosyntax in the generative tradition: The initial stages and beyond. In J. Cabrelli Amaro, et al. (eds), 9-32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giancaspro, D., Halloran, B. & Iverson, M
2014. Examining L3 transfer: The acquisition of differential object marking in L3 Brazilian Portuguese. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI logo
Hermas, A
2010Language acquisition as computational resetting: Verb movement in L3 initial state. International Journal of Multilingualism 7: 343-362. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2014Multilingual transfer: L1 morphosyntax in L3 English. International Journal of Language Studies 8(2): 1-24.Google Scholar
Ionin, T., Montrul, S. & Santos, H
2011An experimental ivestigation of the expression of genericity in English, Spanish and Brazilian Portuguese. Lingua 121: 963-985. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, M
2009N-drop at the initial state of L3 Portuguese: Comparing simultaneous and additive bilinguals of English/Spanish. In Minimalist Inquiries into Child and Adult Language Acquisition: Case Studies Across Portuguese, A. Pires & J. Rothman (eds), 221-244. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2010Informing the age of acquisition debate: L3 as a litmus tes. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Teaching (IRAL) 48(2-3): 221-243.Google Scholar
Jarvis, S. & Pavlenko, A
2008Crosslinguistic Influence in Language and Cognition. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jin, F
2009Third language acquisition of Norwegian objects: Interlanguage transfer or L1 influence? In Leung (ed.), 144-161.Google Scholar
Kellerman, E
1983Now you see it, now you don’t. In Language Transfer in Language Learning, S. Gass & L. Selinker (eds), 112-134. Rowley MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
1986An eye for an eye: crosslinguistic constraints on the development of the L2 lexicon. In Crosslinguistic Influence in Second Language Acquisition, E. Kellerman & M.S. Smith (eds), 35-48. Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Kulundary, V. & Gabriele, A
2012Examining the role of L2 syntactic development in L3 acquisition: A look at relative clauses. In Cabrelli Amaro, et al. (eds), 195-222.Google Scholar
Leung, Y-k. I
(ed) 2009Third Language Acquisition and Universal Grammar. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Llama, R
2013Object clitic (mis)placement in L3 Spanish: Exploring the differences between the English-French and French-English Bilinguals. Paper presented at the II coloquio de estudiantes de estudios hispánicos, Montreal, Canada.
Lozano, C
2002The interpretation of overt and null pronouns in non-native Spanish. In Durham Working Papers in Linguistics, Vol. 8, H. Marsden, S. Pourcel & M. Whong-Bharr (eds), 53-66. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Meisel, J
1983Transfer as a second language strategy. Language and Communication 3: 11-46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S
2006Bilingualism, incomplete acquisition and language change. In L2 Acquisition and Creole Genesis [Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 42], C. Lefebvre, L. White & C. Jourdens (eds), 379-400. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Dias, R. & Santos, H
2011Clitics and object expression in the L3 acquisition of Brazilian Portuguese: Structural similarity matters for transfer. Second Language Research 27: 21-58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Na Ranong, S. & Leung, Y.-k.I
2009Null objects in L1 Thai-L2 English-L3 Chinese: An empirical take on a theoretical problem. In Leung (ed.), 162-191.Google Scholar
Núñez, C
2011The acquisition of existentials in L3 Spanish by speakers of Romanian and Serbo-Croatian. Paper presented at the Hispanic Linguistics Symposium, Athens, Georgia.
Odlin, T
1989Language Transfer: Cross-linguistic Influence in Language Learning. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Özçelik, Ö
2013Selectivity in L3 transfer: Effects of typological and linguistic similarity in the L3 Turkish of Uzbek-Russian bilinguals. Paper presented at the Generative Linguistics in the Old World (GLOW 36), Lund, Sweden.
Paradis, J
2004The relevance of specific language impairment in understanding the role of transfer in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics 25, 67-82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ringbom, H
1978The influence of the mother tongue on the translation of lexical items. Interlanguage Studies Bulletin 3: 80-101.Google Scholar
1987The Role of the First Language in Foreign Language Learning. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
2007Cross-linguistic Similarity in Third Language Learning. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Rothman, J
2010On the typological economy of syntactic transfer: Word order and relative clause high/low attachment preference in L3 Brazilian Portuguese. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Teaching (IRAL) 48(2-3): 245-273.Google Scholar
2011L3 syntactic transfer selectivity and typological determinacy: The typological primacy model. Second Language Research 27(1): 107-127. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2013aCognitive economy, non-redundancy and typological primacy in L3 acquisition: Evidence from initial stages of L3 Romance. In Romance languages and linguistic theory 2011 [Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory 5], S. Baauw, F. Drijkoningen & M. Pinto (eds), 217-248. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2013bLinguistic and cognitive motivations for the Typological Primacy Model (TPM) of third language (L3) transfer: Timing of acquisition and proficiency considered. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J. & Cabrelli Amaro, J
2010What variables condition syntactic transfer?: A look at the L3 initial state. Second Language Research 26(2): 189-218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J., Cabrelli Amaro, J. & de Bot, K
2013Third language acquisition. In The Cambridge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, J. Herschensohn & M. Young-Scholten (eds), 372-393. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J. & Halloran, B
2013Formal linguistic approaches to L3/Ln acquisition: A focus on morphosyntactic transfer in adult multilingualism. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics 33: 51-67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B. & Sprouse, R
1996L2 cognitive states and the Full Transfer/Full Access model. Second Language Research 12: 40-72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Singleton, D. & Little, D
1984[2005]A first encounter with Dutch: Perceived language distance and transfer as factors in comprehension. In Introductory Readings in L3, B. Hufeisen & R.J. Fouser (eds), 101-109. Tübingen: Stauffenberg. First published in Language across Cultures, L. Mac Mathúna & D. Singleton (eds), 259-269. Dublin: Irish Association for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Singleton, D. & Ó Laoire, M
2006Psychotypology and the “L2 factor” in cross-lexical interaction: An analysis of English and Irish influence in learner French. In Spräk, larande och utbildning i sikte, M. Bendtsen, M. Björklund, C. Fant & L. Forsman (eds), 191-205. Vasa: Faculty of Education, Abo Akademi.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. & García Mayo, M.P
2013The L3 syntax-discourse interface. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Torrego, E
1996Experiencers and raising verbs. In Current Issues in Comparative Grammar, R. Freidlin (ed.), 101-120. Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, S. & Hammarberg, B
1998Language switches in L3 production: Implications for a polyglot speaking mode. Applied Linguistics 19: 295-333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wrembel, M
2012Foreign accentedness in third language acquisition. In Cabrelli Amaro, et al. (eds), 281-310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar