From evolutionarily conserved frontal regions for sequence processing to human innovations for syntax
Empirical advances have been made in understanding how human language, in its combinatorial complexity and unbounded expressivity,
may have evolved from the communication systems present in our evolutionary ancestors. However, a number of cognitive processes
and neurobiological mechanisms that support language may not have evolved specifically for communication, but rather from
abilities that support perception and cognition more generally. We review recent evidence from comparative behavioural and
neurobiological studies on structured sequence learning in human and nonhuman primates. These studies support the notion that
certain sequence learning abilities are evolutionarily conserved and engage corresponding inferior frontal brain regions across
the species, regions also involved in processing language in humans. Alongside the cross-species similarities is evidence for
human specialisations, illuminating the likely evolutionary pathways towards language in modern humans. We argue that cognitive
abilities that were in place for animals to learn combinatorial relationships in the sensory world were available and co-opted for
language in humans.
Article outline
- Introduction
- Structured sequence learning and empirical links to analogous operations in language
- Conserved sequence processing in human and nonhuman primates
- Cross-species similarities and differences in sequence learning and the primate frontal cortex
- Possible evolutionary pathways from conserved combinatorial capacities to the emergence of language
- Patterned sequences in motor movements
- Primate vocal communication and the perception of conspecific vocal exchanges
- The expansion of cognitive combinatorial abilities
- Pathways from conserved combinatorial learning to proto-language
- Toward a new roadmap
- Acknowledgements
-
References
References (88)
References
Arbib, M. A. (2012). How the brain got language: The mirror system hypothesis (Vol. 161): OUP USA.
Arbib, M. A. (2016). Towards a computational comparative neuroprimatology: framing the language-ready brain. Physics of life reviews, 161, 1–54.
Arbib, M. A. (2017). Dorsal and ventral streams in the evolution of the language-ready brain: Linking language to the world. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 431, 228–253.
Arnold, K., & Zuberbuhler, K. (2006). Language evolution: semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature, 441(7091), 303.
Bahlmann, J., Schubotz, R. I., & Friederici, A. D. (2008). Hierarchical artificial grammar processing engages Broca’s area. NeuroImage, 42(2), 525–534.
Belin, P., Zatorre, R. J., Lafaille, P., Ahad, P., & Pike, B. (2000). Voice-selective areas in human auditory cortex. Nature, 403(6767), 309–312.
Bergman, T. J., Beehner, J. C., Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2003). Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science, 302(5648), 1234–1236.
Berwick, R. C., Friederici, A. D., Chomsky, N., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2013). Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(2), 89–98.
Bickerton, D., & Szathmary, E. (2009). Biological Foundations and Origin of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bolhuis, J. J., Okanoya, K., & Scharff, C. (2010). Twitter evolution: converging mechanisms in birdsong and human speech. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 11(11), 747–759.
Bozic, M., Tyler, L. K., Ives, D. T., Randall, B., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2010). Bihemispheric foundations for human speech comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 107(40), 17439–17444.
Brauer, J., Anwander, A., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Neuroanatomical prerequisites for language functions in the maturing brain. Cerebral Cortex, 21(2), 459–466.
Chakraborty, M., Walloe, S., Nedergaard, S., Fridel, E. E., Dabelsteen, T., Pakkenberg, B., Bertelsen, M. F., Dorrestein, G. M., Brauth, S. E., Durand, S. E., & Jarvis, E. D. (2015). Core and shell song systems unique to the parrot brain. PLoS One, 10(6), e0118496.
Conway, C. M., & Pisoni, D. B. (2008). Neurocognitive basis of implicit learning of sequential structure and its relation to language processing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145(1), 113–131.
Cope, T. E., Wilson, B., Robson, H., Drinkall, R., Dean, L., Grube, M., Jones, P. S., Patterson, K., Griffiths, T. D., & Rowe, J. B. (2017). Artificial grammar learning in vascular and progressive non-fluent aphasias. Neuropsychologia, 1041, 201–213.
Ding, N., Melloni, L., Zhang, H., Tian, X., & Poeppel, D. (2016). Cortical tracking of hierarchical linguistic structures in connected speech. Nature Neuroscience, 19(1), 158–164.
Endress, A. D., Carden, S., Versace, E., & Hauser, M. D. (2010). The apes’ edge: positional learning in chimpanzees and humans. Animal Cognition, 13(3), 483–495.
Fedorenko, E., Duncan, J., & Kanwisher, N. (2012). Language-selective and domain-general regions lie side by side within Broca’s area. Current Biology, 22(21), 2059–2062.
Fitch, W. T., & Friederici, A. (2012). Artificial grammar learning meets formal language theory: An overview. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 3671, 1933–1955.
Fitch, W. T., & Hauser, M. D. (2004). Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science, 303(5656), 377–380.
Friederici, A. D. (2011). The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiological Reviews, 91(4), 1357–1392.
Friederici, A. D. (2017). Evolution of the neural language network. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 24(1), 41–47.
Friederici, A. D. (2017). Language in our brain: The origins of a uniquely human capacity. MIT Press.
Friederici, A. D., Bahlmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R. I., & Anwander, A. (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: functional localization and structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 103(7), 2458–2463.
Fritz, J., Mishkin, M., & Saunders, R. C. (2005). In search of an auditory engram. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 102(26), 9359–9364.
Gabay, Y., Thiessen, E. D., & Holt, L. L. (2015). Impaired statistical learning in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58(3), 934–945.
Giraud, A. L., & Poeppel, D. (2012). Cortical oscillations and speech processing: emerging computational principles and operations. Nature Neuroscience, 15(4), 511–517.
Gómez, R. (2002). Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science, 13(5), 431–436.
Grube, M., Bruffaerts, R., Schaeverbeke, J., Neyens, V., De Weer, A. -S., Seghers, A., Bergmans, B., Dries, E., Griffiths, T. D., & Vandenberghe, R. (2016). Core auditory processing deficits in primary progressive aphasia. Brain, aww067
Hage, S. R., & Nieder, A. (2013). Single neurons in monkey prefrontal cortex encode volitional initiation of vocalizations. Nature Communications, 41, 2409.
Hauser, M. D., & Glynn, D. (2009). Can Free-Ranging Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Extract Artificially Created Rules Comprised of Natural Vocalizations? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(2), 161–167.
Hecht, E. E., Gutman, D. A., Bradley, B. A., Preuss, T. M., & Stout, D. (2015). Virtual dissection and comparative connectivity of the superior longitudinal fasciculus in chimpanzees and humans. NeuroImage, 1081, 124–137.
Heimbauer, L. A., Conway, C. M., Christiansen, M. H., Beran, M. J., & Owren, M. J. (2018). Visual artificial grammar learning by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): exploring the role of grammar complexity and sequence length. Animal Cognition, 1–18.
Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 8(5), 393–402.
Honda, E., & Okanoya, K. (1999). Acoustical and syntactical comparisons between songs of the white-backed Munia (Lonchura striata) and its domesticated strain, the Bengalese finch (Lonchura striata var. domestica). Zoological Science, 161, 319–326.
Hsu, H. J., Tomblin, J. B., & Christiansen, M. H. (2014). Impaired statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies in adolescents with specific language impairment. Frontiers in psychology, 51.
Hurford, J. R. (2012). The Origins of Grammar: Language in the Light of Evolution II: Oxford University Press.
Hurford, J. R. (2018). Mutation, modularity, merge, communication and selection. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 211, 76–79.
Kikuchi, Y., Attaheri, A., Wilson, B., Rhone, A. E., Nourski, K. V., Gander, P. E., Kovach, C. K., Kawasaki, H., Griffiths, T. D., & Howard Iii, M. A. (2017). Sequence learning modulates neural responses and oscillatory coupling in human and monkey auditory cortex. PLoS Biology, 15(4), e2000219.
Lam, N. H. L., Schoffelen, J. -M., Uddén, J., Hultén, A., & Hagoort, P. (2016). Neural activity during sentence processing as reflected in theta, alpha, beta, and gamma oscillations. NeuroImage, 1421, 43–54.
Locurto, C., Fox, M., & Mazzella, A. (2015). Implicit learning in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) and pigeons (Columba livia). Learning & Behavior, 43(2), 129–142.
Lu, K., & Vicario, D. S. (2014). Statistical learning of recurring sound patterns encodes auditory objects in songbird forebrain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 111(40), 14553–14558.
Makuuchi, M., Bahlmann, J., Anwander, A., & Friederici, A. D. (2009). Segregating the core computational faculty of human language from working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 106(20), 8362–8367.
Mars, R. B., Eichert, N., Jbabdi, S., Verhagen, L., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2018). Connectivity and the search for specializations in the language-capable brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 211, 19–26.
Meyer, T., Ramachandran, R., & Olson, C. R. (2014). Statistical Learning of Serial Visual Transitions by Neurons in Monkey Inferotemporal Cortex. The Journal of neuroscience, 34(28), 9332–9337.
Miller, C. T., Thomas, A. W., Nummela, S. U., & Lisa, A. (2015). Responses of primate frontal cortex neurons during natural vocal communication. Journal of Neurophysiology, 114(2), 1158–1171.
Milne, A. E., Mueller, J. L., Männel, C., Attaheri, A., Friederici, A. D., & Petkov, C. I. (2016). Evolutionary origins of non-adjacent sequence processing in primate brain potentials. Scientific Reports, 61.
Milne, A. E., Petkov, C. I., & Wilson, B. (2017). Auditory and visual sequence learning in humans and monkeys using an artificial grammar learning paradigm. Neuroscience.
Neubert, F. X., Mars, R. B., Thomas, A. G., Sallet, J., & Rushworth, M. F. (2014). Comparison of human ventral frontal cortex areas for cognitive control and language with areas in monkey frontal cortex. Neuron, 81(3), 700–713.
Newport, E. L., Hauser, M. D., Spaepen, G., & Aslin, R. N. (2004). Learning at a distance II. Statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies in a non-human primate. Cogn. Psychol., 49(2), 85–117.
Patel, G. H., Yang, D., Jamerson, E. C., Snyder, L. H., Corbetta, M., & Ferrera, V. P. (2015). Functional evolution of new and expanded attention networks in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(30), 9454–9459.
Pelucchi, B., Hay, J. F., & Saffran, J. R. (2009). Statistical Learning in a Natural Language by 8‐Month‐Old Infants. Child Development, 80(3), 674–685.
Petersson, K. M., Folia, V., & Hagoort, P. (2012). What artificial grammar learning reveals about the neurobiology of syntax. Brain and Language, 120(2), 83–95.
Petkov, C., Kang, X., Alho, K., Bertrand, O., Yund, E., & Woods, D. (2004). Attentional modulation of human auditory cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 7(6), 658–663.
Petkov, C. I., & Jarvis, E. D. (2012). Birds, primates, and spoken language origins: behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 41, 12.
Petkov, C. I., Kayser, C., Steudel, T., Whittingstall, K., Augath, M., & Logothetis, N. K. (2008). A voice region in the monkey brain. Nature Neuroscience, 11(3), 367–374.
Ravignani, A., Sonnweber, R. -S., Stobbe, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2013). Action at a distance: dependency sensitivity in a New World primate. Biology Letters, 9(6), 20130852.
Ravignani, A., & Sonnweber, R. (2017). Chimpanzees process structural isomorphisms across sensory modalities. Cognition, 1611, 74–79.
Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 6(6), 855–863.
Rilling, J. K., Glasser, M. F., Preuss, T. M., Ma, X., Zhao, T., Hu, X., & Behrens, T. E. J. (2008). The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nature Neuroscience, 11(4), 426–428.
Rinne, T., Muers, R. S., Salo, E., Slater, H., & Petkov, C. I. (2017). Functional imaging of audio–visual selective attention in monkeys and humans: How do lapses in monkey performance affect cross-species correspondences? Cerebral Cortex, 27(6), 3471–3484.
Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences, 21(5), 188–194.
Saffran, J., Hauser, M. D., Seibel, R., Kapfhamer, J., Tsao, F., & Cushman, F. (2008). Grammatical pattern learning by human infants and cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Cognition, 107(2), 479–500.
Saffran, J. R. (2002). Constraints on statistical language learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 172–196.
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274(5294), 1926–1928.
Santolin, C., & Saffran, J. R. (2017). Constraints on Statistical Learning Across Species. Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
Schlenker, P., Chemla, E., & Zuberbühler, K. (2016). What Do Monkey Calls Mean? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(12), 894–904.
Schulze, K., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Mishkin, M. (2012). Test of a motor theory of long-term auditory memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(18), 7121–7125.
Scott, B. H., Mishkin, M., & Yin, P. (2012). Monkeys have a limited form of short-term memory in audition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(30), 12237–12241.
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2017). Precursors to language: Social cognition and pragmatic inference in primates. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 24(1), 79–84.
Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Cognition, evolution, and behavior (2nd ed.): Oxford University Press.
Shima, K., Isoda, M., Mushiake, H., & Tanji, J. (2007). Categorization of behavioural sequences in the prefrontal cortex. Nature, 445(7125), 315.
Sonnweber, R., Ravignani, A., & Fitch, W. T. (2015). Non-adjacent visual dependency learning in chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 1–13.
Spierings, M. J., & ten Cate, C. (2016). Budgerigars and zebra finches differ in how they generalize in an artificial grammar learning experiment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 113(27), E3977–E3984.
Stobbe, N., Westphal-Fitch, G., Aust, U., & Fitch, W. T. (2012). Visual artificial grammar learning: comparative research on humans, kea (Nestor notabilis) and pigeons (Columba livia). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 1995–2006.
Taglialatela, J. P., Russell, J. L., Schaeffer, J. A., & Hopkins, W. D. (2008). Communicative signaling activates ‘Broca’s’ homolog in chimpanzees. Current Biology, 18(5), 343–348.
ten Cate, C. (2017). Assessing the uniqueness of language: Animal grammatical abilities take center stage. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 24(1), 91–96.
ten Cate, C. (2018). The comparative study of grammar learning mechanisms: birds as models. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 211, 13–18.
Tu, H. -W., & Dooling, R. J. (2012). Perception of warble song in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus): evidence for special processing. Animal Cognition, 15(6), 1151–1159.
Van Essen, D. C., & Dierker, D. L. (2007). Surface-based and probabilistic atlases of primate cerebral cortex. Neuron, 56(2), 209–225.
Vernaleo, B. A., & Dooling, R. J. (2011). Relative salience of envelope and fine structure cues in zebra finch song. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129(5), 3373–3383.
Wang, L., Uhrig, L., Jarraya, B., & Dehaene, S. (2015). Representation of numerical and sequential patterns in macaque and human brains. Current Biology, 25(15), 1966–1974.
Wilson, B., Kikuchi, Y., Sun, L., Hunter, D., Dick, F., Smith, K., Thiele, A., Griffiths, T. D., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Petkov, C. I. (2015a). Auditory sequence processing engages evolutionarily conserved regions of frontal cortex in macaques and humans. Nature Communications, 6(8901).
Wilson, B., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Petkov, C. I. (2017). Conserved sequence processing in primate frontal cortex. Trends in Neurosciences.
Wilson, B., & Petkov, C. I. (2017). Relational knowledge and the origins of language. In R. Seyfarth, D. L. Cheney & M. L. Platt (Eds.), The social origins of language. Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press.
Wilson, B., Slater, H., Kikuchi, Y., Milne, A. E., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., Smith, K., & Petkov, C. I. (2013). Auditory artificial grammar learning in macaque and marmoset monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(48), 18825–18835.
Wilson, B., Smith, K., & Petkov, C. I. (2015b). Mixed-complexity artificial grammar learning in humans and macaque monkeys: evaluating learning strategies. European Journal of Neuroscience, 41(5), 568–578.
Zimmerer, VC, Cowell, PE, Varley, RA (2014) Artificial grammar learning in individuals with severe aphasia. Neuropsychologia 531:25–38.
Cited by (2)
Cited by two other publications
Arbib, Michael A., Francisco Aboitiz, Judith M. Burkart, Michael C. Corballis, Gino Coudé, Erin Hecht, Katja Liebal, Masako Myowa-Yamakoshi, James Pustejovsky, Shelby S. Putt, Federico Rossano, Anne E. Russon, P. Thomas Schoenemann, Uwe Seifert, Katerina Semendeferi, Chris Sinha, Dietrich Stout, Virginia Volterra, Sławomir Wacewicz & Benjamin Wilson
Arbib, Michael A., Francisco Aboitiz, Judith M. Burkart, Michael Corballis, Gino Coudé, Erin Hecht, Katja Liebal, Masako Myowa-Yamakoshi, James Pustejovsky, Shelby Putt, Federico Rossano, Anne E. Russon, P. Thomas Schoenemann, Uwe Seifert, Katerina Semendeferi, Chris Sinha, Dietrich Stout, Virginia Volterra, Sławomir Wacewicz & Benjamin Wilson
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 6 august 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.