This paper outlines a first attempt to model the special constraints that arise in language processing in conversation, and to explore the implications such functional considerations may have on language typology and language change. In particular, we focus on processing pressures imposed by conversational turn-taking and their consequences for the cultural evolution of the structural properties of language. We present an agent-based model of cultural evolution where agents take turns at talk in conversation. When the start of planning for the next turn is constrained by the position of the verb, the stable distribution of dominant word orders across languages evolves to match the actual distribution reasonably well. We suggest that the interface of cognition and interaction should be a more central part of the story of language evolution.
Altmann, G. T. M. (1999). Thematic role assignment in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 411, 124–45.
Altmann, G. T. M. (2004) Language-mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: The ‘blank screen paradigm’. Cognition, 931, 79–87.
Austin, P. (2001). Word order in a free word order language: the case of Jiwarli. In J. Simpson, D. Nash, M. Laughren, P. Austin, & B. Alpher (Eds.), Forty years on: Ken Hale and Australian languages (pp. 205–323). Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.
Barthel, M., Sauppe, S., Levinson, S. C., & Meyer, A. S. (2016). The timing of utterance planning in task-oriented dialogue: Evidence from a novel list-completion paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 71, 1858.
Bentz, C., & Christiansen, M. H. (2010). Linguistic adaptation at work? The change of word order and case system from Latin to the Romance languages. In A. Smith, M. Schouwstra, B. de Boer, & K. Smith (Eds.), Proceedings of the eighth international conference on the evolution of language (pp. 26–33). London: World Scientific.
Bowern, C. (2012). A grammar of Bardi. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
Blasi, D. E., Wichmann, S., Hammarström, H., Stadler, P. F., & Christiansen, M. H. (2016). Sound–meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(39): 10818–23.
Bögels, S., Kendrick, K. H., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). Never Say No… How the brain interprets the pregnant pause in conversation. PloS One, 10(12), e0145474.
Bögels, S., & Levinson, S. C. (2016). The brain behind the response: Insights into turn-taking in conversation from neuroimaging. Research on Language and Social Interaction. Advance online publication.
Bögels, S., Magyari, L., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). Neural signatures of response planning occur midway through an incoming question in conversation. Scientific Reports, 51, 12881.
Christensen, P., Fusaroli, R., & Tylén, K. (2016). Environmental constraints shaping constituent order in emerging communication systems: Structural iconicity, interactive alignment and conventionalization. Cognition, 1461, 67–80.
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(5), 489–509.
Croft, W. (2000). Explaining language change: An evolutionary approach. Harlow, Essex: Longman.
Dingemanse, M., Roberts, S. G., Baranova, J., Blythe, J., Drew, P., Floyd, S., Gisladottir, R. S., Kendrick, K. H., Levinson, S. C., Manrique, E., Rossi, G., & Enfield, N. J. (2015). Universal principles in the repair of communication problems. PloS One, 10(9), e0136100.
Donders, F. C. (1868). La vitesse des actes psychiques. Archives Néerlandaise, 31, 269–317.
Dryer, M. S. (2013a). Order of subject, object and verb. In Matthew S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Available online: [URL], Accessed on 2017-01-11.
Dryer, M. S. (2013b). Position of polar question particles. In Matthew S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Available online: [URL], Accessed on 2017-01-13.
Dunn, M., Greenhill, S. J., Levinson, S. C., & Gray, R. D. (2011). Evolved structure of language shows lineage-specific trends in word-order universals. Nature, 473(7345), 79–82.
Duranti, A. (1981). The Samoan fono: A sociolinguistic study (No. 80). Pacific Linguistics Series B, 801. Department of Linguistics, Research School of Pacific Studies, Australian National University.
Enfield, N. J. (2008). Language as shaped by social interaction. Behavioral and brain sciences, 31(5), 519–520.
Fedzechkina, M., Jaeger, T. F., & Newport, E. L. (2012). Language learners restructure their input to facilitate efficient communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(44), 17897–17902.
Ferrer-i Cancho, R. (2008). Some word order biases from limited brain resources: A mathematical approach. Advances in Complex Systems, 11(3), 393–414.
Ferrer-i-Cancho, R. (2014). Why might SOV be initially preferred and then lost or recovered? A theoretical framework. In E. A. Cartmill, S. Roberts, H. Lyn, & H. Cornish (Eds.), The evolution of language – Proceedings of the 10th International Conference (EVOLANG10), Evolution of Language Conference (Evolang 2014) (pp. 66–73). Vienna, Austria, April 14–17.
Ferrer-i-Cancho, R. (2015). The placement of the head that minimizes online memory. A complex systems approach. Language Dynamics and Change, 5(1), 114–137.
Ferrer-i-Cancho, R. (2016) Kauffman's adjacent possible in word order evolution. In S. G. Roberts, C. Cuskley, L. McCrohon, L. Barceló-Coblijn, O. Fehér, & T. Verhoef (Eds.),
The evolution of language: Proceedings of the 11th International Conference (EVOLANG11)
. Available online: [URL].
Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2009). Joint action, interactive alignment, and dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 292–304.
Gell-Mann, M., & Ruhlen, M. (2011). The origin and evolution of word order. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(42), 17290–17295.
Gibson, E., Piantadosi, S. T., Brink, K., Bergen, L., Lim, E., & Saxe, R. (2013). A noisy-channel account of crosslinguistic word-order variation. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1079–1088.
Gisladottir, R. S., Chwilla, D., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). Conversation electrified: ERP correlates of speech act recognition in underspecified utterances. PLoS One, 10(3), e0120068.
Givón, T. (1983b). Topic continuity in discourse: An Introduction. In T. Givón (Ed.), Topic continuity in discourseTopic continuity in discourse: A quantitative cross-language study. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Goldin-Meadow, S., So, W. C., Özyürek, A., & Mylander, C. (2008). The natural order of events: How speakers of different languages represent events non- verbally. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(27), 9163–9168.
Haspelmath, M., Dryer, M. S., Gil, D., & Comrie, B. (2008). World Atlas of Language Structures. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library. Available online: [URL]. Accessed on 2013-04-18.
Hawkins, J. (1994). A performance theory of order and constituency. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hawkins, J. A. (2004). Efficiency and complexity in grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hick, W. E. (1952). On the rate of gain of information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4(1), 11–26.
Horn, L. (1984). Toward a new taxonomy for pragmatic inference: Q-based and R-based implicature. In D. Schiffrin (Ed.), Meaning, form, and use in context: Linguistic applications (pp. 11–42). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Reprinted in Kasher (ed., 1998), vol. IV: 389–418.
Indefrey, P. (2011). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components: A critical update. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(255), 1–16.
Jaeger, T. F. (2010). Redundancy and reduction: Speakers manage syntactic information density. Cognitive Psychology, 61(1), 23–62.
Jaeger, T. F., & Levy, R. P. (2006). Speakers optimize information density through syntactic reduction. In P. B. Schölkopf, J. C. Platt, & T. Hoffman (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing systems 19 (pp. 849–856). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kamide, Y. (2008). Anticipatory processes in sentence processing. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2(4), 647–670.
Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye-movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 491, 133–159.
Kendrick, K. H. (2012). Particles at the transition space. Talk presented at the
Interactional Foundations of Language Meeting
, Kreuth, Germany, 2012-11-01–2012-11-02.
Kendrick, K. H. (2015). The intersection of turn-taking and repair: The timing of other-initiations of repair in conversation. Frontiers in psychology, 61, 250.
Kendrick, K. H., & Torreira, F. (2015). The timing and construction of preference: A quantitative study. Discourse Processes, 52(4), 255–289.
Kirby, S., Griffiths, T., & Smith, K. (2014). Iterated learning and the evolution of language. Current opinion in neurobiology, 281, 108–114.
Knoeferle, P., Crocker, M. W., Scheepers, C., & Pickering, M. J. (2005). The influence of the immediate visual context on in cremental thematic role assignment: Evidence from eye-movements in depicted events. Cognition, 951, 95–127.
Krupa, V. (1982). Syntactic typology and linearization. Language, 58(3), 639–645.
Langus, A., & Nespor, M. (2010). Cognitive systems struggling for word order. Cognitive Psychology, 60(4), 291–318.
Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1–38.
Levinson, S. (2006). On the human interaction engine. In N. Enfield & S. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and human interaction (pp. 39–69). Oxford: Berg.
Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Levinson, S. C. (2016). Turn-taking in human communication–origins and implications for language processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(1), 6–14.
Lupyan, G., & Christiansen, M. H. (2002). Case, word order, and language learnability: Insights from connectionist modeling. In Proceedings of the
24th annual conference of the cognitive science society
(pp. 596–601). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Maurits, L., Perfors, A., & Navarro, D. (2010). Why are some word orders more common than others? A uniform information density account. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 231, 1585–1593.
Mithun, M. (1992). Is basic word order universal? In D. Payne (Ed.), Pragmatics of word order flexibility (pp. 15–62). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Nowak, I., & Baggio, G. (2016). The emergence of word order and morphology in compositional languages via multigenerational signaling games. Journal of Language Evolution, 1(2), 137–150.
Ochs, E. (1982). Ergativity and word order in Samoan child language. Language, 58(3), 646–671.
Ochs, E., Schegloff, E. A., & Thompson, S. A. (1996). Interaction and grammar, Vol. 131. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Piantadosi, S. T., Tily, H., & Gibson, E. (2011). Word lengths are optimized for efficient communication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(9), 3526–3529.
Roberts, F., Margutti, P., & Takano, S. (2011). Judgments concerning the valence of inter-turn silence across speakers of American English, Italian, and Japanese. Discourse Processes, 48(5), 331–354.
Roberts, S. G., Torreira, F., & Levinson, S. C. (2015). The effects of processing and sequence organisation on the timing of turn taking: A corpus study. Frontiers in Psychology, 61, 509.
Roberts, S., & Winters, J. (2013). Linguistic diversity and traffic accidents: Lessons from statistical studies of cultural traits. PloS One, 8(8), e70902.
de Ruiter, J. P., Mitterer, H., & Enfield, N. J. (2006). Projecting the end of a speaker's turn: A cognitive cornerstone of conversation. Language, 82(3), 515–535.
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 40(4), 696–735.
Sauppe, S. (2016). Verbal semantics drives early anticipatory eye movements during the comprehension of verb-initial sentences. Frontiers in Psychology, 71, 95.
Schegloff, E. A. (1989). Reflections on language, development, and the interactional character of talk-in-interaction. In M. Bornstein & J. S. Bruner (Eds.), Interaction in human development (pp. 139–153). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Schegloff, E. A., Jefferson, G., & Sacks, H. (1977). The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language, 531, 361–382.
Schouwstra, M., & de Swart, H. (2014). The semantic origins of word order. Cognition, 131(3), 431–436.
Steels, L., & Belpaeme, T. (2005). Coordinating perceptually grounded categories through language: a case study for colour. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(4), 469–489.
Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., Hoymann, G., Rossano, F., De Ruiter, J. P., Yoon, K.-E., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(26), 10587–10592.
Swartz, S. (1987) Pragmatic structure and word order in Warlpiri. Papers in Australian linguistics, 171, 151–166.
Tamariz, Mónica, et al.. (2017). “The Interactive Origin of Iconicity.” Cognitive Science.
Tanaka, H. (2000). Turn projection in Japanese talk-in-interaction. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 33(1), 1–38.
Tanaka, H. (2005). Grammar and the “timing” of social action: Word order and preference organization in Japanese. Language in Society, 34(3), 389–430.
Tomlin, R. S. (1986). Basic word order: Functional principles. London: Croom Helm.
Van Valin, R. D., & LaPolla, R. J. (1997). Syntax: Structure, meaning, and function. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Verhoef, T., Roberts, S. G. & Dingemanse, M. (2015). Emergence of systematic iconicity: transmission, interaction and analogy.
In Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society
, 2481–2486. Cognitive Science Society. [URL]
Zipf, G. K. (1949). Human behavior and the principle of least effort. Oxford, England: Addison-Wesley Press.
Cited by (11)
Cited by 11 other publications
Rühlemann, Christoph & Mathias Barthel
2024. Word frequency and cognitive effort in turns-at-talk: turn structure affects processing load in natural conversation. Frontiers in Psychology 15
Trujillo, James P. & Judith Holler
2024. Information distribution patterns in naturalistic dialogue differ across languages. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 31:4 ► pp. 1723 ff.
Enfield, N. J.
2022. Enchrony. WIREs Cognitive Science 13:4
Motamedi, Yasamin, Kathryn Montemurro, Natasha Abner, Molly Flaherty, Simon Kirby & Susan Goldin-Meadow
2022. The Seeds of the Noun–Verb Distinction in the Manual Modality: Improvisation and Interaction in the Emergence of Grammatical Categories. Languages 7:2 ► pp. 95 ff.
Shi, Da, Yawen Shan, Zhiyue Zhao & Shuhan Zhang
2022. Identification and Influence of Tourism Rituals: Analysis of Eye Movement Recognition of Tourism Images. Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research 46:1 ► pp. 147 ff.
Dingemanse, Mark
2020. Resource-rationality beyond individual minds: the case of interactive language use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 43
2019. Language as Mechanisms for Interaction: Towards an Evolutionary Tale. In Language, Logic, and Computation [Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 11456], ► pp. 209 ff.
Slonimska, Anita & Seán G. Roberts
2017. A case for systematic sound symbolism in pragmatics: Universals in wh-words. Journal of Pragmatics 116 ► pp. 1 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 20 november 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.