Article published In:
ITL - International Journal of Applied Linguistics
Vol. 165:1 (2014) ► pp.4675
References (37)
Borganovo, C., Bruhn de Garvito, J., & Prévost, P. (2008). Methodological issues in the L2 acquisition of a syntax/semantics phenomenon: How to assess L2 knowledge of mood in Spanish relative clauses. In J. Bruhn de Garavito, & E. Valenzuela (Eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 10th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 13–24). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Borganovo, C., & Prévost, P. (2003). Knowledge of polarity subjunctive in L2 Spanish. In B. Beachley, A. Brown, & F. Conlon (Eds.), BUCLD 2: Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Dvelopment (pp. 150–161). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Casella, G., & Berger R. L. (2001). Statistical inference. Pacific Grove, CA: Duxbury Press.Google Scholar
Chen, H. (2010). Contrastive learner corpus analysis of epistemic modality in interlanguage pragmatic competence in L2 writing. Arizona Working Papers in SLA & Teaching, 171, 27–51.Google Scholar
Collentine, J. (1995a). The development of subjunctive and complex-syntactic abilities among FL Spanish learners. Hispania, 78(1), 122–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1995b). The development of complex syntax and mood-selection abilities by intermediate-level learners of Spanish. Hispania, 781, 123–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foster, D. (1982). Internal contradictions of a Spanish subjunctive. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 20(2), 131 – 137.Google Scholar
Givón, T. (1994). Irrealis and the subjunctive. Studies in Language, 18(2), 265–337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldin, M. (1974). A psychological perspective of the Spanish subjunctive. Hispania, 57(2), 295–301. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gudmestad, A. (2006). L2 variation and the Spanish subjunctive: Linguistic features predicting mood selection. In C. Klee & T. Face (Eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 7th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages (pp. 170–184). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Haznadar, B. (2003). Missing surface inflection in adult & child L2 acquisition. In Liceras, Juana, Helmut Zobl, & Helen Goodluck (Eds.), Proceedings of the 6th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference , (pp. 140–149). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Hualde, J.I., Olarrea, A., Escobar, A.M., Travis, C. (2001). Introducción a la lingüística hispánica. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Iverson, M., Kempchinsky, P., & Rothman, J. (2008). Interface vulnerability and knowledge of the subjunctive/indicative distinction with negated epistemic predicates in L2 Spanish. EUROSLA Yearbook, 81,135–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kellerman, E. (1985). If at first you do succeed. In S. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 345–353). Rowly, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. (2007). Acquiring (or assembling) functional categories in second language acquisition. In A. Belikova, L. Meroni, & M. Umeda (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2nd Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America , 233–244. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Lazano, A. (1995). Cognitive development, deontic and epistemic subjunctives. Hispanic Linguistics, 6/71, 94–115.Google Scholar
Lightbown, P. (1985). Great expectations: Second-language acquisition research and classroom teaching. Applied Linguistics, 61, 173–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lightbown, P., & Spada, N. (2006). How languages are learned. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Lubbers Quesada, M. (1998). L2 acquisition of the Spanish subjunctive mood and prototype schema development. SAL, 2(1), 1–23.Google Scholar
Massery, L., & Fuentes, C. (2012). The role of functional categories in L2 Spanish: Persistence of L1 CP values in IL. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 5(2), 297. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mejias-Vikindi E. (1994). Assertion and Speaker’s Intention: A pragmatically based account of mood in Spanish. Hispania, 77(4), 892–902. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. (1998). The Acquisition of mood selection in Spanish relative clauses. Journal of Child Language, 25(3), 585–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L. (2000b). Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second Language Research, 16(2), 103–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ramsey F.L., & Schafer D.W. (2012). The statistical sleuth: A course in methods of data analysis. St. Paul-Minneapolis MN: Brooks/Cole Publishing.Google Scholar
Schane, S. (1994). Illocutionary verbs, subject responsibility, and presupposition: The indicative versus the subjunctive in Spanish. In P. Hashemipour, R. Maldonado, & M. Van Naerssen (Eds.), Studies in language learning and Spanish linguistics in honor of Tracy D. Terrell (pp. 361–374). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B., & Sprouse, R. (1996). L2 cognitive states and the full transfer/full access model. Second Language Research, 121, 40–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Searle, S. (1987). Linear models for unbalanced data. New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R. (2009). What is easy and what is hard to acquire in a second language. In M. Bowles, T. Ionin, S. Montrul, & A. Tremblay (Eds.), Proceedings of the 10th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference , (pp. 280–293). Somerville, MA: Casacadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. (2000). Syntactic optionality in non-native grammars. Second Language Research, 161, 293–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2005). Selective optionality in language development. In L. Cornips & K. Corrigan (Eds.), Syntax and variation: Reconciling the biological and the social (pp. 55–80). Amsterdam: Johns Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Studerus, L. (1995). Some unresolved issues in Spanish mood use. Hispania, 78(1), 94–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Terrell, T. & Hooper, J. (1974). A semantically based analysis of mood in Spanish. Hispania, 571, 484–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Terrell, T., Baycroft, B., & Perrone, C. (1987). The subjunctive in Spanish interlanguage: Accuracy and comprehensibility. In B. VanPatten, T. Dvorak, & J. Lee (Eds.), Foreign language learning: A research perspective (pp. 19–31). NewYork, NY: Newbury.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I.M., & Sorace, A. (2006). Differentiating interfaces: L2 performance in syntax semantics and syntax discourse phenomena. In D. Bamman, T. Magnitskaia, & C. Zaller (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, BUCLD 30 , 653–664. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Vainikka, A., & Young-Scholten, M. (1996a). Gradual development of L2 phrase structure. Second Language Research, 121, 7–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1996b). The early stages in adult L2 syntax: Additional evidence from romance speakers. Second Language Research, 121, 140–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L. (2003). Second language acquisition and universal grammar. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (2)

Cited by two other publications

Mañas Navarrete, I., E. Rosado Villegas, S. Mujcinovic & N. Fullana Rivera
2024. Acquisition of modal readings of the Imperfect tense in L2 Spanish. Language Acquisition  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Antonova-Unlu, Elena & Li Wei
2020. Examining possible sources of L2 divergence at the pragmatics interface: Turkish accusative in the end-state grammar of L1 Russian and L1 English users of L2 Turkish. Lingua 244  pp. 102868 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 4 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.