Article published In:
Journal of Second Language Studies
Vol. 1:1 (2018) ► pp.530
References
Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K.
(1998) Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 381, 419–439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G. T. M.
(2011) Language can mediate eye movement control within 100 milliseconds, regardless of whether there is anything to move the eyes to. Acta Psychologica, 1371, 190–200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I., & Ramscar, M.
(2012) Granularity and the acquisition of grammatical gender: How order-of-acquisition affects what gets learned. Cognition, 1221, 292–305. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bordag, D., & Pechmann, T.
(2007) Factors influencing L2 gender processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 101, 299–314. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caffarra, S., Janssen, N., & Barber, H. A.
(2014) Two sides of gender: ERP evidence for the presence of two routes during gender agreement processing. Neuropsychologia, 631, 124–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cooper, R. M.
(1974) The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 84–107. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corbett, G. G.
(1991) Gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Caramazza, A., & Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2000) The cognate facilitation effect: Implications for models of lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 261, 1283–1296.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Kovacic, D., Frank, J., & Caramazza, A.
(2003) On the autonomy of the grammatical gender systems of the two languages of a bilingual. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 61, 181–200. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dahan, D., Swingley, D., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Magnuson, J. S.
(2000) Linguistic gender and spoken-word recognition in French. Journal of Memory and Language, 421, 465–480. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B. & Nas, G. L. J.
(1991) Lexical representation of cognates and noncognates in compound bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(1), 90–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duchon, A., Perea, M., Sebastián-Galles, N., Martí, A., & Carreiras, M.
(2013) EsPal: One-stop shopping for Spanish Word properties. Behavior Research Methods, 451, 1246–1258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Valdés Kroff, J. R., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., & Gerfen, C.
(2013) When gender and looking go hand in hand. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 351, 353–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Chambers, S. M.
(1973) Lexical access in naming time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 121, 627–635. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gillon Dowens, M., Vergara, M., Barber, H. A., & Carreiras, M.
(2009) Morphosyntactic processing in late second-language learners. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 221, 1870–1887. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H. & Frost, R.
(2001) Two routes to grammatical gender: Evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(6), 627–651. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, B.
(1983) Lexical access and lexical decision: Mechanisms of frequency sensitivity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 221, 24–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Griebling McCowen, L., & Alvord, S. M.
(2006) Mi mamá es bonito: Acquisition of Spanish gender by native English speakers. In C. A. Klee & T. L. Face (Eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 7th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages (pp. 161–169). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A.
(2012) Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem. Second Language Research, 281, 191–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., Kotz, S. A., Hofmann, J., Valentin, V. V., Dapretto, M., & Bookheimer, S. Y.
(2004) The neural correlates of grammatical gender decisions in Spanish. NeuroReport, 151, 863–866. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hockett, C. F.
(1958) A course in modern linguistics. New York, NY: Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H.
(2013) Grammatical gender in adult L2 acquisition: Relations between lexical and syntactic variability. Second Language Research, 291, 33–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Cross-linguistic lexical and syntactic co-activation in L2 sentence processing. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(1), 96–130. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F.
(2008) Cognate effects in picture naming: Does cross-language activation survive a change of script? Cognition, 1061, 501–511. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Smith, S.
(2016) Do grammatical-gender distinctions learned in the second language influence native-language lexical processing? International Journal of Bilingualism, 201, 30–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kousta, S. T., Vinson, D. P., Vigliocco, G.
(2008) Investigating linguistic relativity through bilingualism: The case of grammatical gender. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 341, 843–858Google Scholar
Lee, E., Lu, D., & Garnsey, S.
(2013) L1 word order and sensitivity to verb bias in L2 processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 161, 761–775. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lemhöfer, K., Spalek, K., & Schriefers, H.
(2008) Cross-language effects of grammatical gender in bilingual word recognition and production. Journal of Memory and Language, 591, 312–330. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Libben, M. R. & Titone, D. A.
(2009) Bilingual lexical access in context: Evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(2), 381–390.Google Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A.
(2007) Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 181, 193–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Real-time processing of gender marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 631, 447–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, M. C.
(1993) The interaction of lexical and syntactic ambiguity. Journal of Memory and Language, 321, 692–715. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 41, 1–16. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S.
(1994) The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, 101(4), 676–703. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Potowski, K.
(2007) Command of gender agreement in school-age Spanish-English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 111, 301–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morales, L., Paolieri, D., Dussias, P. E., Valdés Kroff, J. R., Gerfen, C., & Bajo, M. T.
(2015) The gender congruency effect during bilingual spoken-word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 191, 294–310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paolieri, D., Cubelli, R., Macizo, P., Bajo, T., Lotto, L., & Job, R.
(2010) Grammatical gender processing in Italian and Spanish bilinguals. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 631, 1631–1645. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, M.
(1997) The cognitive neuropsychology of bilingualism. In A. M. B. de Groot & J. F. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 331–354). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M.
(1991) The acquisition of gender: What Spanish children tell us. Child Language, 181, 571–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sabourin, L., Stowe, L. A., & de Haan, G. J.
(2006) Transfer effect in learning a second language grammatical gender system. Second Language Research, 221, 1–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sabourin, L., & Stowe, L. A.
(2008) Second language processing: When are first and second languages processed similarly? Second Language Research, 241, 397–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanchez-Casas, R. M., Davis, C. W., & Garcia-Albea, J. E.
(1992) Bilingual lexical processing: Exploring the cognate/non-cognate distinction. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 4(4), 293–310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saslow, M. G.
(1967) Latency for Saccadic Eye Movement. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 571, 1030–1033. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schilling, H. E. H., Rayner, K., & Chumbley, J. I.
(1998) Comparing naming, lexical decision, and eye fixation times: Word frequency effects and individual differences. Memory & Cognition, 261, 1270–1281. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, A. I. & Kroll, J. F.
(2006) Bilingual lexical activation in sentence context. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(2), 197–212. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, A. I., Kroll, J. F., & Diaz, M.
(2007) Reading words in Spanish and English: Mapping orthography to phonology in two languages. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(1), 106–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sherkina-Leiber, M.
(2004) The cognate facilitation effect in bilingual speech processing: The case of Russian-English bilingualism. Cahiers linguistiques d’Ottawa, 321, 108–121.Google Scholar
Valdés Kroff, J. R., Dussias, P. E., Gerfen, C., Perrotti, L., & Bajo, M. T.
(2017) Experience with code-switching modulates the use of grammatical gender during sentence processing. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 71, 163–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Assche, E., Duyck, W., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Diependaele, K.
(2009) Does bilingualism change native-language reading? Cognate effects in a sentence context. Psychological Science, 20(8), 923–927. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Hell, J. G. & Dijkstra, T.
(2002) Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 91, 780–789. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska-McGregor, M., & Leung, Y. I.
(2004) Gender and number agreement in nonnative Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 251, 105–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., & Coltheart, M.
(2003) Speed of lexical and nonlexical processing in French: The case of the regularity effect. Psychonomic Bulleting & Review, 101, 947–953. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 7 other publications

Beatty-Martínez, Anne L. & Paola E. Dussias
2019. Revisiting Masculine and Feminine Grammatical Gender in Spanish: Linguistic, Psycholinguistic, and Neurolinguistic Evidence. Frontiers in Psychology 10 DOI logo
Casado, Alba, Pilar Ferré & Daniela Paolieri
2023. Gender congruency between languages influence second-language comprehension: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Neurolinguistics 68  pp. 101156 ff. DOI logo
Garrido-Pozú, Juan J.
2022. Predictive processing of grammatical gender: Using gender cues to facilitate processing in Spanish. Lingua 278  pp. 103416 ff. DOI logo
Gudmestad, Aarnes, Amanda Edmonds & Thomas Metzger
2019. Using Variationism and Learner Corpus Research to Investigate Grammatical Gender Marking in Additional Language Spanish. Language Learning 69:4  pp. 911 ff. DOI logo
Lozano-Argüelles, Cristina & Nuria Sagarra
2021. Interpreting experience enhances the use of lexical stress and syllabic structure to predict L2 word endings. Applied Psycholinguistics 42:5  pp. 1135 ff. DOI logo
Rojas, Jennifer Markovits
2022. Morphosyntactic Features Versus Morphophonological Features in L2 Gender Acquisition: A Cross-Language Perspective. Languages 7:2  pp. 142 ff. DOI logo
Sossouvi, Laurent-Fidèle & Mei-Chih Lin
2020. La flexión nominal en la producción escrita de estudiantes benineses del profesorado de ELE. IKASTORRATZA.e-journal on Didactics  pp. 61 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 3 may 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.