Phonological short-term memory capacity and L2 oral performance
Phonological short-term memory (PSTM) capacity is the ability to retain verbal information briefly (Archibald
& Gathercole, 2006). Since PSTM facilitates the storage of verbal material, one expects a positive relationship between PSTM
and several aspects of second language (L2) learning. Some studies have found a relationship between PSTM and oral production
skills, such as fluency (e.g.,
O’Brien et al., 2006). This paper reports on the results
of a study that looked at the inter-relations between complexity, accuracy, and fluency (CAF), and between these dimensions and
PSTM. One hundred and thirty-five L2 learners of Spanish participated in the study. Results showed that performance in one L2
speech dimension did not have a negative impact on performance in another dimension, but that as complexity increased or
decreased, there was a better chance of fluency than accuracy also increasing or decreasing. PSTM capacity was related to
complexity and fluency, but not to accuracy. These results were interpreted as lending some support to Skehan’s Trade-off
Hypothesis (
Skehan, 2009).
Article outline
- Theoretical background
- The relationship between complexity, accuracy, and fluency
- The working memory system
- CAF and memory capacity
- Present study
- Methodology
- Participants
- Instruments
- Oral production task
- Phonological short-term memory test
- Procedure
- Scoring
- Oral production task
- Phonological short-term memory test
- Results
- Discussion
- Conclusion
- Acknowledgements
- Notes
-
References
References (54)
References
Atkins, P. W. B., & Baddeley, A. D. (1998). Working memory and distributed vocabulary learning. Applied psycholinguistics, 191, 537–552.
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 361, 537–552.
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates.
Conway, A. R. A., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A review and a user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 121, 769–786.
Cho, M. (2018). Task complexity, modality, and working memory in L2 task performance. System, 721, 85–98.
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 191, 450–466.
Ellis, N. C. (1996). Sequencing in SLA: Phonological memory, chunking and points of order. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 181, 91–126.
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 111, 19–23.
Foster, P., Bolibaugh, C., & Kotula, A. (2014). Knowledge of nativelike selections in an L2: The influence of exposure, memory, age of onset and motivation in foreign language and immersion settings. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 361, 101–132.
Foster, P., & Skehan, P. (1996). The influence of planning and task type on second language performance. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 181, 299–323.
Foster, P., Tonkyn, A., & Wigglesworth, G. (2000). Measuring spoken language: A unit for all reasons. Applied Linguistics, 211, 354–375.
Freed, B. (2000). Is fluency, like beauty, in the eyes (and ears) of the beholder? In H. Riggenbach (Ed.), Perspectives on fluency (pp. 243–265). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
Gilabert, R. (2007). The simultaneous manipulation of task complexity along planning time and +/− here-and-now: Effects on L2 oral production. In M. P. Garcia Mayo (Ed.), Investigating tasks in a formal language learning. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Gilabert, R., & Munoz, C. (2010). Differences in attainment and performance in a foreign language: The role of working memory capacity. International Journal of English Studies, 101, 19–42.
Goo, J. (2012). Corrective feedback and working memory capacity in interaction-driven L2 learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 341, 445–474.
Granena, G. (2019). Cognitive aptitudes and L2 speaking proficiency: Links between LLAMA and Hi-LAB. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 411, 313–336.
Guiraud, P. (1954). Les charactères statistiques du vocabulaire. Essai de méthodologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Harrington, M., & Sawyer, M. (1992). L2 working memory capacity and L2 reading skill. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 141, 25–38.
Housen, A., Kuiken, F., & Vedder, I. (2012). Complexity, accuracy and fluency: Definitions, measurement and research. In A. Housen, F. Kuiken, & I. Vedder (Eds.), Dimensions of L2 performance and proficiency. Complexity, accuracy and fluency in SLA (pp. 1–19). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Ishikawa, T. (2008). The effects of task demands of intentional reasoning on L2 speech performance. The Journal of Asia TEFL, 51, 29–63.
Jackson, D. O., & Suethanapornkul, S. (2013). The cognition hypothesis: A synthesis and meta-analysis of research on second language task complexity. Language Learning, 631, 330–367.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R. A., Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Variation in working memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control. In A. R. A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. N. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 21–48). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kormos, J., & Trebits, A. (2011). Working memory capacity and narrative task performance. In P. Robinson, J. M. Norris, M. Bygate, & K. Van Den Branden (Eds.), Second language task complexity: Researching the cognition hypothesis of language learning and performance (pp. 267–285). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Kuiken, F., & Vedder, I. (2007). Task complexity and measures of linguistic performance in L2 writing. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 451, 261–284.
Levelt, W. J. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Levelt, W. (1999). ‘Language production: A blueprint for the speaker’ In C. Brown and P. Hagoort (Eds.), Neurocognition of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Li, S. (2013). The interactions between the effects of implicit and explicit feedback and individual differences in language analytic ability and working memory. Modern Language Journal, 971, 634–654.
Linck, J., Hughes, M., Campbell, S., Silbert, N., Tare, M., Jackson, S., et al. (2013). Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning, 631, 530–566.
Malvern, D. D., & Richards, B. J. (1997). A new measure of lexical diversity. In A. Ryan & A. Wray (Eds.), Evolving models of language (pp. 58–71). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Meara, P. (2005). LLAMA language aptitude tests. Swansea: Lognostics.
Michel, M., Kuiken, F., & Vedder, I. (2007). The influence of complexity in monologic versus dialogic tasks in Dutch L2. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 451, 241–259.
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (1998). Individual differences in second language proficiency: Working memory as language aptitude. In A. F. Healey & L. J. Bourne (Eds.), Foreign language learning: Psycholinguistic studies on training and retention. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Nielson, K. B. (2014). Can planning time compensate for individual differences in working memory capacity? Language Teaching Research, 181, 272–293.
O’Brien, I., Segalowitz, N., Collentine, J., & Freed, B. (2006). Phonological memory and lexical narrative, and grammatical skills in second language oral production by adult learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 271, 377–402.
Ortega, L. (1995). Planning and second language oral performance (Unpublished MA thesis). University of Hawai’i, Honolulu.
Payne, J. S., & Whitney, P. J. (2002). Developing L2 oral proficiency through synchronous CMC: Output, working memory, and interlanguage development. Calico Journal, 201, 7–32.
Robinson, P. (2001). Task Complexity, cognitive resources, and syllabus design: A triadic framework for examining task influences on SLA. In P. Robinson, M. H. Long, & J. C. Richards (Eds.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 287–318). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Robinson, P. (2003). Attention and memory during SLA. In C. J. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 631–678). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Skehan, P. (1998). A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Skehan, P. (2009). Modelling second language performance: Integrating complexity, accuracy, fluency, and lexis. Applied Linguistics, 301, 510–532.
Skehan, P., & Foster, P. (1997). Task type and task processing conditions as influences on foreign language performance. Language Teaching Research, 11, 185–211.
Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 871, 245–251.
Tagarelli, K. M., Borges Mota, M., & Rebuschat, P. (2015). Working memory, learning conditions, and the acquisition of L2 syntax. In Z. Wen, M. Borges Mota, & A. McNeill (Eds.), Working memory in second language acquisition and processing (pp. 224–247). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research Methods, 371, 498–505.
Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2016). How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between first and second language learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1411, 65–82.
Vermeer, A. (2000). Coming to grips with lexical richness in spontaneous speech data. Language Testing, 171, 65–83.
Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. (1996). The capacity theory of sentence comprehension: Critique of Just and Carpenter (1992). Psychological Review, 1031, 761–772.
Wen, Z. (2016). Phonological and executive working memory in L2 task-based speech planning and performance. Language Learning Journal, 441, 418–435.
Williams, J. N., & Lovatt, P. (2003). Phonological memory and rule learning. Language Learning, 531, 177–233.
Yilmaz, Y. (2013). Relative effects of explicit and implicit feedback: The role of working memory capacity and language analytic ability. Applied Linguistics, 341, 344–368.
Zuniga, M., & Simard, D. (2019). Factors influencing L2 self-repair behavior: The role of L2 proficiency, attentional control and L1 self-repair behavior. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 481, 43–59.
Cited by (2)
Cited by two other publications
Jiang, Michael Yi-Chao, Morris Siu-Yung Jong, Na Wu, Bin Shen, Ching-Sing Chai, Wilfred Wing-Fat Lau & Biyun Huang
2022.
Integrating Automatic Speech Recognition Technology Into Vocabulary Learning in a Flipped English Class for Chinese College Students.
Frontiers in Psychology 13
Suzuki, Yuichi
2021.
Individual differences in memory predict changes in breakdown and repair fluency but not speed fluency: A short-term fluency training intervention study.
Applied Psycholinguistics 42:4
► pp. 969 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 10 august 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.