Part of
Structures, Strategies and Beyond: Studies in honour of Adriana Belletti
Edited by Elisa Di Domenico, Cornelia Hamann and Simona Matteini
[Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today 223] 2015
► pp. 8396
References (18)
References
Adani, Flavia. 2008. The Role of Features in Relative Clause Comprehension: A Study of Typical and Atypical Development, Ph.D. dissertation, University of Milano-Bicocca.
. 2010. Re-thinking the acquisition of relative clauses in Italian: Towards a grammatically-based account. Journal of Child Language 22: 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Belletti Adriana, Friedmann, Naama, Brunato, Dominique & Rizzi, Luigi. 2012. Does gender make a difference? Comparing the effect of gender on children’s comprehension of relative clauses in Hebrew and Italian, Lingua 122(10): 1053–1064. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, João & Lobo, Maria. 2014. Testing relativized minimality in intervention effects:Tthe comprehension of relative clauses with complex DPs in European Portuguese. Ms, FCSH-Universidade Nova de Lisboa.
Costa, João & Pereira, Sandra. 2013. A gente. Pronominal status and agreement revisited. The Linguistic Review 30: 161–184. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, João, Friedmann, Naama, Silva, Carolina & Yachini, Maya. 2013. The boy that the chef cooked. Acquisition of PP relatives in European Portuguese and Hebrew. To appear in Lingua DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, João, Grillo, Nino & Lobo, Maria. 2012. Minimality beyond lexical restriction: Processing and acquisition of headed and free wh-dependencies in European Portuguese. Revue Roumaine de Linguistique 57(2): 143–160.Google Scholar
Costa, João, Lobo, Maria, & Silva, Carolina. 2011. Subject-object asymmetries in the acquisition of relative clauses in European Portuguese: Adults vs. children. Lingua 121: 1083–1100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Den Dikken, Marcel. 2001. “Pluringulars”, pronouns and quirky agreement. The Linguistic Review 18: 19–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, Naama & Costa, João. 2010. The child heard a coordinated sentence and wondered: On children’s difficulty in understanding coordination and relative clauses with crossing dependencies. Lingua 120(6): 1502–1515. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedmann, Naama, Belletti, Adriana & Luigi, Rizzi. 2009. Relativized relatives: Types of intervention in the acquisition of A-bar dependencies. Lingua 119: 67–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grillo, Nino. 2005. Minimality effects in agrammatic comprehension. In Proceedings of ConSOLE XIII, Sylvia Blaho, Eric Schoorlemmer & Luis Vicente (eds), 106–120. <[URL]>Google Scholar
. 2008. Generalized minimality. Ph.D. dissertation, Utrecht. Institute of Linguistics OTS.
Menuzzi, Sérgio 2000. First person plural anaphora in Brazilian Portuguese: Chains and constraint interaction in binding. In Portuguese Syntax: New Comparative Studies, Costa Jõao (ed.), 191–240. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Novogrodsky, Rama & Friedmann, Naama. 2006. The production of relative clauses in syntactic SLI: A window to the nature of the impairment. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology 8(4): 364–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Panagiotidis, Phoevos. 2002. Pronouns, Clitics and Empty Nouns: ‘Pronominality’ and Licensing in Syntax [Linguistik Aktuell/Linguistics Today 46]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva, Carolina. In preparation. Interpretation of Strong, Clitic and Null Pronouns in the Acquisition of European Portuguese. Ph.D. dissertation, Universidade Nova de Lisboa.
Taylor, Michael. 2009. On the pronominal status of Brazilian Portuguese a gente. In NYU Working Papers in Linguistics, Vol 2: Papers in Syntax Patricia Irwin & Violeta Vázquez Rojas Maldonado (eds) 1–36. New York NY: New York University.Google Scholar