On the processing of Free Indirect Discourse
First results and methodological challenges
In this contribution we report on the results from two psycholinguistic experiments investigating the processing of Free Indirect Discourse (FID). We conceive of FID as a linguistic means that cues comprehenders to take over the perspective of a protagonist in third-person narrations. Using both on-line and off-line measures, we tested the hypothesis that the referent of the protagonist receives a higher activation status during reading if his or her thoughts are related through FID. The FID cues we used were questions and discourse particles. In addition, we compared different inferential statistic procedures in the analysis of the results. Although the cues that were employed as FID markers in the experimental materials had an influence on the perception of narrative perspective, no indication was found for the hypothesis that narrative perspective mediated through FID influences the salience of the protagonist during reading. We discuss the implications of this null result and point to some more general methodological problems arising in the investigation of processing of literary text.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.Theoretical background
- 3.Experimental evidence
- 3.1Materials
- 3.2Pilot 1: Off-line questionnaire study
- 3.3Pilot 2: Expert rating
- 3.4Experiment 1: Self-paced reading
- 3.4.1Experiment 1: Procedure
- 3.4.2Experiment 1: Design and predictions
- 3.4.3Experiment 1: Results
- Data treatment
- Proportion of correct answers
- Reading Times per Word in Target Sentence
- Response latencies
- 3.4.4Experiment 1: Discussion
- 3.5Experiment 2: Self-paced reading and memory test
- 3.5.1Materials
- 3.5.2Procedure
- 3.5.3Participants
- 3.5.4Experiment 2: Design and predictions
- 3.5.5Experiment 2: Results
- Data treatment
- Proportion of correct answers
- Reading times per word in target sentence
- Response latencies
- Memory test
- 3.5.6Experiment 2: Discussion
- 4.General discussion
-
Acknowledgements
-
Note
-
References
References (34)
References
Ariel, M. 2001. Accessibility theory: An overview. In Text Representation: Linguistic and Psycholinguistic Aspects [Human Cognitive Processing 8], T. Sanders, J. Schilperoord, & W. Spooren (eds), 29–87. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Asher, N. & Lascarides, A. 2003. Logics of Conversation. Cambridge: CUP.
Banfield, A. 1973. Narrative style and the grammar of direct and indirect speech. Foundations of Language 10(1): 1–39.
Banfield, A. 1982. Unspeakable Sentences. Boston MA: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Barr, D. 2008. Analyzing ‘visual world’ eyetracking data using multilevel logistic regression. Journal of Memory and Language 59(4): 457–474.
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C. & Tily, H. J. 2013. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68(3): 255–278.
Bortolussi, M. & Dixon, P. 2003. Psychonarratology: Foundations for the Empirical Study of Literary Response. Cambridge: CUP.
Bransford, J. D., Barclay, J. R. & Franks, J. J. 1972. Sentence memory: A constructive versus interpretive approach. Cognitive Psychology 3(2): 193–209.
Bray, J. 2007. The ‘dual voice’ of free indirect discourse: A reading experiment. Language and Literature 16(1): 37–52.
Clark, H. H. 1973. The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 12(4): 335–359.
Eckardt, R. 2014. The Semantics of free indirect discourse. How Texts Allow to Mindread and Eavesdrop. Leiden: Brill.
Fludernik, M. 1993. The Fictions of Language and the Languages of Fiction. Oxford: Taylor & Francis.
Garnham, A. 1981. Mental models as representations of text. Memory & Cognition 9(6): 560–565.
Genette, G. 1994. Die Erzählung. Stuttgart: UTB.
Gundel, J. K., Hedberg, N. & Zacharski, R. 1993. Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language 69(2): 274–307.
Harris, J. A. 2012. Processing Perspectives. PhD dissertation, University of Massachusetts Amherst.
Harris, J. A. 2014. Shifting Viewpoints and Discourse Economy. Poster presented at the 27th annual CUNY conference on human sentence processing, Ohio State University, USA.
Hegarty, M. & Waller, D. 2004. A dissociation between mental rotation and perspective-taking spatial abilities. Intelligence 32(2): 175–191.
Hewitt, L. 1995. Anaphor in subjective contexts in narrative fiction. In Deixis in Narrative: A Cognitive Science Perspective, G. B. J. F. Duchan & L. Hewitt (eds), 325–339. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Holler, A. & Irmen, L. 2007. Empirically assessing the effects of the right frontier constraint. In Anaphora: Analysis, Algorithms and Applications. DAARC 2007. LNAI-Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence, A. Branco (ed.), 15–27. Berlin: Springer.
Jaeger, T. F. 2008. Categorical data analysis: Away from anovas (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language 59(4): 434–446.
Kaiser, E. & Cohen, A. 2012. Free indirect discourse and perspective-taking. Poster presented at the annual conference Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP), Riva del Garda, Italy. https://dl.[URL]
Kintsch, W. 1998. Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge, MA: CUP.
László, J. 1986. Same story with different point of view. SPIEL 5: 1–22.
Long, D. L. 1994. The effects of pragmatics and discourse style on recognition memory for sentences. Discourse Processes 17(2): 213–234.
Maier, E. 2012. Switches between direct and indirect speech in ancient Greek. Journal of Greek Linguistics 12(1): 118–139.
Maier, E. 2014. Mixed quotation: The grammar of apparently transparent opacity. Semantics and Pragmatics 7: 1–67.
Nieuwenhuis, R., Pelzer, B. & te Grotenhuis, M. 2012. influence.me. Tools for Detecting Influential Data in Mixed Effects Models. Version 0.9.2.
Sanford, A. J., Moar, K. & Garrod, S. C. 1988. Proper names as controllers of discourse focus. Language and Speech 31(1): 43–56.
Schlenker, P. 2004. Context of thought and context of utterance: A note on free indirect discourse and the historical present. Mind & Language 19(3): 279–304.
Sharvit, Y. 2008. The puzzle of free indirect discourse. Linguistics and Philosophy 31(3): 353–395.
Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D. & Simonsohn, U. 2011. False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science 22(11): 1359–1366.
Yao, B. & Scheepers, C. 2011. Contextual modulation of reading rate for direct versus indirect speech quotations. Cognition 121(3): 447–453.
Cited by (3)
Cited by three other publications
Harris, Jesse A.
2021.
Extended Perspective Shift and Discourse Economy in Language Processing.
Frontiers in Psychology 12
Bimpikou, Sofia
2020.
Who Perceives? Who Thinks? Anchoring Free Reports of Perception and Thought in Narratives.
Open Library of Humanities 6:2
Holler, Anke
2019.
Alles eine Frage der Perspektive – Zur sogenannten erlebten Rede im narrativen Text.
Zeitschrift für germanistische Linguistik 47:1
► pp. 28 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 27 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.