Article published In:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 10:2 (2020) ► pp.153183
References
Anderson, R. T.
(1999) Loss of gender agreement in L1 attrition: Preliminary results. Bilingual Research Journal, 23(4), 389–408. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001) Lexical morphology and verb use in child first language loss. A preliminary case study investigation. International Journal of Bilingualism, 51, 377–401. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Au, T., Knightly, L., Jun, S., & Oh, J.
(2002) Overhearing a language during childhood. Psychological Science, 13(3), 238–243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H.
(2008) Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chan, A., Matthews, S., & Yip, V.
(2011) The acquisition of relative clauses in Cantonese and Mandarin. In E. Kidd (Ed.), The Acquisition of Relative Clauses. Processing, Typology and Function (pp. 197–225). Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, J. & Shirai, Y.
(2015) The acquisition of relative clauses in spontaneous child speech in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language, 421, 394–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Croft, W.
(2001) Radical Construction Grammar: Syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A.
(2012) Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax proper: Interrogative subject-verb inversion in heritage Spanish, International Journal of Bilingualism, 17(1), 71–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A., Pérez-Tattam, R., Barajas, E., Miller, L., & Sadowski, C.
(2013) The development of tense and aspect morphology in child and adult heritage Spanish: Implications for heritage language pedagogy. In S. John (Ed.), Innovative research and practices in second language acquisition and bilingualism (pp.193–220). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Döpke, S.
(1998) Competing language structures: the acquisition of verb placement by bilingual German-English children. Journal of Child Language, 25(3), 555–584. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, L. W.
(1991) When learning a second language means losing the first. Early Childhood research Quarterly, 61, 323–346. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flores, C. & Barbosa, P.
(2012) When reduced input leads to delayed acquisition: a study on the acquisition of clitic placement by Portuguese heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 18(3), 304–325. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Foroodi Nejad, F., & Paradis, J.
(2009) Crosslinguistic transfer in the acquisition of compound words of Persian-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 121, 411–427. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C.
(2003) DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 351, 116–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. M.
(2007) Miami and North Wales, so far and yet so near: A constructivist account of morphosyntactic development in bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 101, 224–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. M., & Hoff, E.
(2007) Input and the acquisition of language: Three questions. In E. Hoff & M. Schatz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development (pp. 107–127). Malden, MA: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E.
(1995) Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hoff, E.
(2006) How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 261, 55–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Senor, M., & Parra, M.
(2012) Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 391, 1–27. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Hsu, C.
(2014) The role of age in Mandarin-speaking children’s performance of relative clauses. Concentric: Studies in Linguistics, 29–54.Google Scholar
Jia, G., & Aaronson, D.
(2003) A longitudinal study of Chinese children and adolescent learning English in the United States. Applied Psycholinguistics, 241, 131–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jia, G., Aaronson, D., Young, D., Chen, S., & Wagner, J.
(2005) Bilingual Mandarin-, Russian- and Spanish-English speakers’ grammatical proficiency in their two languages. In M. Minami, H. Kobayashi, M. Nakayama, & H. Sirai (Eds.), Studies in language sciences (pp. 160–171). Tokyo: Kurosio.Google Scholar
Jia, G.
(2008) Heritage language development, maintenance, and attrition among recent Chinese immigrants in New York City. In A. W. He & Y. Xiao (Eds.), Chinese as a heritage language: Fostering rooted world citizenry (pp.189–203). Honolulu, HA: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Jia, R., & Paradis., J.
(2015) The use of referring expressions in narratives by Mandarin heritage language children and the role of language environment factors in predicting individual differences. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 181, 737–752. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Kaufman, D., & Aronoff, M.
(1991) Morphological disintegration and reconstruction in first language attrition. In H. Seliger & R. Vago (eds.), First Language Attrition (pp.175–188). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, D.
(2005) Acquisition, attrition, and revitalization of Hebrew in immigrant children. In D. Ravid & H. Bat-ZeevShyldkrot (Eds.), Perspective on language and language development (pp. 407–418). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, E., Chen, A., & Chiu, J.
(2015) Cross-linguistic influence in simultaneous Cantonese – English bilingual children’s comprehension of relative clauses. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 181, 438–452. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kondo-Brown, K.
(2005) Differences in Language skills: Heritage language learner subgroups and foreign language learners. The Modern Language Journal, 891, 563–581. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. W.
(2000) A dynamic usage-based model. In M. Barlow & S. Kemmer (Eds.), Usage-based models of language (pp. 1–63). Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Li, W., & Lee, S.
(2001) L1 development in an L2 environment: The use of Cantonese classifiers and quantifiers by young British-born Chinese in Tyneside. International Journal of Bilingual Education and bilingualism, 41, 359–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S.
(2002) Incomplete acquisition and attrition of Spanish tense/aspect distinctions in adult bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 51, 39–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Second language acquisition and first language loss in adult early bilinguals: exploring some differences and similarities. Second Language Research, 211, 199–249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Knowledge of tense-aspect and mood in Spanish heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 131, 239–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) The acquisition of heritage languages. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., & Ionin, T.
(2012) Dominant language transfer in Spanish heritage speakers and second language learners in the interpretation of definite article. The Modern Language Journal, 96 (1), 70–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E.
(2002) What’s the difference between ‘toilet paper’ and ‘paper toilet’? French-English bilingual children’s crosslinguistic transfer in compound nouns. Journal of Child Language, 291, 1–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Grady, W., Lee, M., & Choo, M.
(2001) The acquisition of relative clauses by heritage and non-heritage learners of Korean as a second language: A comparative study. Journal of Korean Language Education, 121, 283–294.Google Scholar
O’Grady, W., Kwak, H., Lee, O., & Lee, M.
(2011) An emergentist perspective on heritage language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 331, 223–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J.
(2010) Bilingual children’s acquisition of English verb morphology: Effects of language exposure, structure complexity, and task type. Language Learning, 601, 651–680. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, P., Tremblay, A. & Crago, M.
(2014) French-English bilingual children’s sensitivity to child-level and language-level input factors in morphosyntactic acquisition. In T. Grüter, & J. Paradis (Eds.), Input and Experience in Bilingual Development (pp.161–180). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pires, A., & Rothman, J.
(2009) Disentangling sources of incomplete acquisition: An explanation for competence divergence across heritage grammars. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), 211–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M.
(2006) Incomplete acquisition: American Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics, 141, 191–262.Google Scholar
(2008) Gender under incomplete acquisition: Heritage speakers” knowledge of noun categorization. Heritage Language Journal, 6(1), 40–71.Google Scholar
(2011) Reanalysis in Adult Heritage Language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(2), 305–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J.
(2007) Heritage speaker competence differences, language change and input type: Inflected infinitives in heritage Brazilian Portuguese. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11(4), 359–389. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Understanding the Nature and Outcomes of Early Bilingualism: Romance Languages as Heritage Languages. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2) 155–164. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rinke, E., & Flores, C.
(2014) Morphosyntactic knowledge of clitics by Portuguese heritage bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 171, 681–699. DOI logo.Google Scholar
Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., Filiaci, F., & Baldo, M.
(2009) Bilingual children’s sensitivity to specificity and genericity: Evidence from metalinguistic awareness. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 121, 239–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C.
(1994) Language Contact and Change: Spanish in Los Angeles. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Yip, V. & Matthews, S.
(2007) The Bilingual Child: early development and language contact. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 14 other publications

Coşkun Kunduz, Aylin & Silvina Montrul
2024. Relative clauses in child heritage speakers of Turkish in the United States. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 14:2  pp. 218 ff. DOI logo
Daskalaki, Evangelia, Vasiliki Chondrogianni & Elma Blom
2023. Path and rate of development in child heritage speakers: Evidence from Greek subject/object form and placement. International Journal of Bilingualism 27:5  pp. 634 ff. DOI logo
Daskalaki, Evangelia, Adriana Soto-Corominas, Aisha Barisé, Johanne Paradis, Xi Chen & Alexandra Gottardo
2023. The interplay between syntactic and morphological comprehension in heritage contexts: The case of relative clauses in heritage Syrian Arabic. Applied Psycholinguistics 44:6  pp. 1043 ff. DOI logo
Hao, Jiuzhou & Vasiliki Chondrogianni
2023. Comprehension and production of non-canonical word orders in Mandarin-speaking child heritage speakers. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 13:4  pp. 468 ff. DOI logo
Hu, Shenai, Francesca Costa & Maria Teresa Guasti
2020. Mandarin–Italian Dual-Language Children’s Comprehension of Head-Final and Head-Initial Relative Clauses. Frontiers in Psychology 11 DOI logo
Mai, Ziyin, Lucy Zhao & Virginia Yip
2022. The Mandarin ba-construction in school-age heritage speakers and their parental input. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 12:3  pp. 377 ff. DOI logo
PARADIS, Johanne
2023. Sources of individual differences in the dual language development of heritage bilinguals. Journal of Child Language 50:4  pp. 793 ff. DOI logo
Paradis, Johanne, Adriana Soto-Corominas, Xi Chen & Alexandra Gottardo
2020. How language environment, age, and cognitive capacity support the bilingual development of Syrian refugee children recently arrived in Canada. Applied Psycholinguistics 41:6  pp. 1255 ff. DOI logo
Paradis, Johanne, Adriana Soto-Corominas, Evangelia Daskalaki, Xi Chen & Alexandra Gottardo
2021. Morphosyntactic Development in First Generation Arabic—English Children: The Effect of Cognitive, Age, and Input Factors over Time and across Languages. Languages 6:1  pp. 51 ff. DOI logo
Requena, Pablo E & Melisa Dracos
2021. Spanish copula selection with adjectives in school-aged bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism 25:3  pp. 548 ff. DOI logo
Shang, Mengyao, Lucy Zhao, Virginia Yip & Ziyin Mai
2024. Protracted development in the heritage lexicon. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism DOI logo
SOTO-COROMINAS, Adriana, Evangelia DASKALAKI, Johanne PARADIS, Magdalena WINTERS-DIFANI & Redab AL JANAIDEH
2022. Sources of variation at the onset of bilingualism: The differential effect of input factors, AOA, and cognitive skills on HL Arabic and L2 English syntax. Journal of Child Language 49:4  pp. 741 ff. DOI logo
Tsoi, Elaine Yee Ling, Wenchun Yang, Angel Chan & Evan Kidd
2019. Mandarin–English speaking bilingual and Mandarin speaking monolingual children’s comprehension of relative clauses. Applied Psycholinguistics 40:4  pp. 933 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 2 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.