We investigated whether the features of the second language (L2) matter when we consider the consequence of short-term L2
immersion on performance in the native language (L1). We compared L1 performance in English-speaking learners of a typologically-dissimilar
L2-Chinese immersed in Chinese while living in Beijing, China and learners of a typologically-similar L2 (Spanish or French) exposed to the
L2 in a classroom setting only. The groups were matched on cognitive abilities. Each group performed a battery of language tasks in English
that assessed the ability to produce and recognize spoken words, as well as to name written words and pseudo-words in the native language.
Immersed learners produced fewer words in their native language, made more semantic errors, and benefited more from higher lexical frequency
when retrieving L1 words relative to classroom learners. Immersed learners also revealed reduced competition from dense phonological
neighborhoods when listening to English words presented in noise, but no difference in English word reading and phonemic decoding
performance compared to classroom learners. Results are consistent with the view that L2 immersion reduces access to the native language,
but suggest that the consequences of L2 immersion on the L1 may be dependent upon the form of cross-language differences.
Ameel, E., Malt, B. C., Storms, G., & Van Assche, F. (2009). Semantic convergence in the bilingual lexicon. Journal of Memory and Language, 601, 270–290.
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 591, 390–412.
Baron, J., & Strawson, C. (1976). Use of orthographic and word-specific knowledge in reading words aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 386–393.
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 681, 255–278.
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., Bojesen Christensen, R. H., Singmann, H., … Green, P. (2016). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1–11. [URL]. Retrieved from [URL]
Baus, C., Costa, A., & Carreiras, M. (2013). On the effects of second language immersion on first language production. Acta Psychologica, 1421, 402–409.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G. (2008a). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 341, 859–873.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G. (2008b). Lexical access in bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 211, 522–538.
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 451, 459–464.
Bice, K., & Kroll, J. F. (in press). English only? Monolinguals in linguistically diverse contexts have an edge in language learning. Brain and Language.
Bice, K., & Kroll, J. F. (2015). Native language change during early stages of second language learning. Neuroreport, 261, 966–971.
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2007). Constraints on parallel activation in bilingual spoken language processing: Examining proficiency and lexical status using eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes, 221, 633–660.
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2013). Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 251, 547–567.
Borodkin, K., & Faust, M. (2014). Native language phonological skills in low-proficiency second language learners. Language Learning, 641, 132–159.
Botezatu, M. R., Landrigan, J.-F., Chen, Q., & Mirman, D. (2015). Phonological neighborhood density modulates errors in spoken word recognition. Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.
Botezatu, M. R., & Mirman, D. (2019). Impaired lexical selection and fluency in post-stroke aphasia. Aphasiology, 331, 667–688.
Bradlow, A. R., & Pisoni, D. B. (1999). Recognition of spoken words by native and non-native listeners: Talker-, listener-, and item-related factors. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1061, 2074–2085.
Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kucera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 411, 977–990.
Canseco-Gonzalez, E., Brehm, L., Brick, C. A., Brown-Schmidt, S., Fischer, K., & Wagner, K. (2010). Carpet or carcel: The effect of age of acquisition and language mode on bilingual lexical access. Language and Cognitive Processes, 251, 669–705.
Chang, C. B. (2012). Rapid and multifaceted effects of second-language learning on first-language speech production. Journal of Phonetics, 401, 249–268.
Chang, C. B. (2013). A novelty effect in phonetic drift of the native language. Journal of Phonetics, 411, 520–533.
Charles-Luce, J., & Luce, P. A. (1990). Similarity neighbourhoods of words in young children’s lexicons. Journal of Child Language, 17(01), 205.
Coltheart, M., & Rastle, K. (1994). Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual route models of reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 201, 1197–1211.
Dąbrowska, E. (2018). Experience, aptitude and individual differences in native language ultimate attainment. Cognition, 1781, 222–235.
De Cara, B., & Goswami, U. (2003). Phonological neighbourhood density: effects in a rhyme awareness task in five-year-old children. Journal of Child Language, 301, 695–710.
Degani, T., Prior, A., & Tokowicz, N. (2011). Bidirectional transfer: The effect of sharing a translation. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 231, 18–28.
Dussias, P. E., & Sagarra, N. (2007). The effect of exposure on syntactic parsing in Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 101, 101–116.
Ecke, P., & Hall, C. J. (2013). Tracking tip-of-the-tongue states in a multilingual speaker: Evidence of attrition or instability in lexical systems?International Journal of Bilingualism, 171, 734–751.
Frenck-Mestre, C. (1993). Use of orthographic redundancies and word identification speed in bilinguals. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 221, 397–409.
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 581, 787–814.
Gollan, T. H., Weissberger, G. H., Runnqvist, E., Montoya, R. I., & Cera, C. M. (2012). Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A Multilingual Naming Test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 151, 594–615.
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
Hartshorne, J. K., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Pinker, S. (2018). A critical period for second language acquisition: Evidence from 2/3 million English speakers. Cognition, 1771, 263–277.
Ide, N., & Suderman, K. (2004). The American National Corpus First Release. Paper presented at the Language resources and evaluation conference (lrec), Lisbon.
Imai, S., Walley, A. C., & Flege, J. E. (2005). Lexical frequency and neighborhood density effects on the recognition of native and Spanish-accented words by native English and Spanish listeners. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1171, 896–907.
Ivanova, I., & Costa, A. (2007). Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production?Acta Psychologica, 1271, 277–288.
Jared, D. (1997). Spelling-sound consistency affects the naming of high-frequency words. Journal of Memory and Language, 361, 505–529.
Jared, D. (2002). Spelling-sound consistency and regularity effects in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 461, 723–750.
Jared, D., & Kroll, J. F. (2001). Do bilinguals activate phonological representations in one or both of their languages when naming words?Journal of Memory and Language, 441, 2–31.
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 211, 60–99.
Kertesz, A. (1982). Western Aphasia Battery. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., & Hoshino, N. (2014). Two languages in mind: Bilingualism as a tool to investigate language, cognition, and the brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 231, 159–163.
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T. (2008). Language selection in bilingual speech: evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica, 1281, 416–430.
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bice, K., & Perrotti, L. (2015). Bilingualism, mind, and brain. Annual Review of Linguistics, 11, 377–394.
Kroll, J. F., Michael, E., Tokowicz, N., & Dufour, R. (2002). The development of lexical fluency in a second language. Second Language Research, 181, 137–171.
Lacruz, I., & Folk, J. R. (2004). Feedforward and feedback consistency effects for high-and low-frequency words in lexical decision and naming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 57A1, 1261–1284.
Lemhöfer, K., Spalek, K., & Schriefers, H. (2008). Cross-language effects of grammatical gender in bilingual word recognition and production. Journal of Memory and Language, 591, 312–330.
Levy, B. J., McVeigh, N. D., Marful, A., & Anderson, M. C. (2007). Inhibiting your native language: The role of retrieval-induced forgetting during second-language acquisition. Psychological Science, 181, 29–34.
Linck, J. A., Hughes, M. M., Campbell, S. G., Silbert, N. H., Tare, M., Jackson, S. R., … Doughty, C. J. (2013). Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning, 631, 530–566.
Linck, J. A., Kroll, J. F., & Sunderman, G. (2009). Losing access to the native language while immersed in a second language: Evidence for the role of inhibition in second-language learning. Psychological Science, 201, 1507–1515.
Linck, J. A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J. T., & Bunting, M. (2013). Working memory and second language learning, comprehension, and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 211, 861–883.
Linck, J. A., & Weiss, D. J. (2015). Can working memory and inhibitory control predict second language learning in the classroom?SAGE Open, 5(4).
Lü, C. (2017). The roles of Pinyin skill in English-Chinese biliteracy learning: Evidence from Chinese immersion learners. Foreign Language Annals, 501, 306–322.
Liu, I., Wu, J. T., & Chou, T. L. (1996). Encoding operation and transcoding as the major loci of the frequency effect. Cognition, 591, 149–168.
Luce, P. A. (1986). Neighborhoods of words in the mental lexicon. Retrieved from Bloomington:
Luce, P. A., & Large, N. R. (2001). Phonotactics, density, and entropy in spoken word recognition. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(5–6), 565–581.
Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear & Hearing, 191, 1–36.
Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 1141, 29–41.
Magnuson, J. S., Dixon, J. A., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2007). The dynamics of lexical competition during spoken word recognition. Cognitive Science, 311, 133–156.
Malins, J. G., & Joanisse, M. F. (2010). The roles of tonal and segmental information in Mandarin spoken word recognition: An eyetracking study. Journal of Memory and Language, 621, 407–420.
Malins, J. G., & Joanisse, M. F. (2012). Setting the tone: An ERP investigation of the influences of phonological similarity on spoken word recognition in Mandarin Chinese. Neuropsychologia, 501, 2032–2043.
Marian, V., Spivey, M., & Hirsch, J. (2003). Shared and separate systems in bilingual language processing: Converging evidence from eyetracking and brain imaging. Brain and Language, 86(1), 70–82.
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Boukrina, O. V. (2008). Sensitivity to phonological similarity within and across languages. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 371, 141–170.
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing Language Profiles in Bilinguals and Multilinguals. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 501, 940.
Marian, V., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). Cross-linguistic transfer and borrowing in bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 281, 369–390.
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 61, 97–115.
Misra, M., Guo, T., Bobb, S. C., & Kroll, J. F. (2012). When bilinguals choose a single word to speak: Electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 671, 224–237.
Nosarti, C., Mechelli, A., Green, D. W., & Price, C. J. (2010). The impact of second language learning on semantic and nonsemantic first language reading. Cerebral Cortex, 201, 315–327.
Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2005). Orthography to phonology and meaning: Comparisons across and within writing systems. Reading and Writing, 181, 193–210.
Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (1998). The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 241, 101–118.
Plat, R., Lowie, W., & De Bot, K. (2018). Word Naming in the L1 and L2: A Dynamic Perspective on Automatization and the Degree of Semantic Involvement in Naming. Frontiers in Psychology, 81, 2256.
R Development Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from [URL]
Raupach, M. (1980). Temporal variables in first and second language speech production. In H. W. Dechert & M. Raupach (Eds.) Temporal variables in speech (pp. 49–60). The Hague: Mouton.
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Araujo, K., Weekes, V. A., Caracciolo, V., Padilla, M., & Ostrosky-Solí, F. (2000). Verbal fluency and repetition skills in healthy older Spanish-English bilinguals. Applied Neuropsychology, 71, 17–24.
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Salvatierra, J., Marquez, M., Matos, L., & Weekes, V. A. (2002). A cross-linguistic comparison of verbal fluency tests. International Journal of Neuroscience, 112(6), 759–776.
Rüschemeyer, S.-A., Nojack, A., & Limbach, M. (2008). A mouse with a roof? Effects of phonological neighbors on processing of words in sentences in a non-native language. Brain and Language, 1041, 132–144.
Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., & Castles, A. (2013). Phonological decoding or direct access? Regularity effects in lexical decisions of grade 3 and 4 children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 661, 338–346.
Schwartz, A. I., Kroll, J. F., & Diaz, M. (2007). Reading words in Spanish and English: Mapping orthography to phonology in two languages. Language and Cognitive Processes, 221, 106–129.
Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implications for learning to read. Child Development, 741, 27–47.
Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 511, 300–304.
Snyder, H. R., Banich, M. T., & Munakata, Y. (2014). All competition is not alike: Neural mechanisms for resolving underdetermined and prepotent competition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 261, 2608–2623.
Spivey, M. J., & Marian, V. (1999). Cross talk between native and second languages: Partial activation of an irrelevant lexicon. Psychological Science, 101, 281–284.
Stanovich, K. E., & Bauer, D. W. (1978). Experiments on the spelling-to-sound regularity effect in word recognition. Memory and Cognition, 61, 410–415.
Stein, M., Dierks, T., Brandeis, D., Wirth, M., Strik, W., & Koenig, T. (2006). Plasticity in the adult language system: A longitudinal electrophysiological study on second language learning. Neuroimage, 331, 774–783.
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Tsai, P. T. (2007). The effects of phonological neighborhoods on spoken word recognition in Mandarin Chinese. Master’s thesis at University of Maryland, College Park.
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent?Journal of Memory and Language, 281, 127–154.
Van Orden, G. C. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & Cognition, 151, 181–198.
Wang, W., Li, X., Ning, N., & Zhang, J. X. (2012). The nature of the homophone density effect: An ERP study with Chinese spoken monosyllable homophones. Neuroscience Letters, 5161, 67–71.
Waters, G. S., Seidenberg, M. S., & Bruck, M. (1984). Children’s and adults’ use of spelling-sound information in three reading tasks. Memory and Cognition, 121, 293–305.
Weber, A., & Cutler, A. (2004). Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 501, 1–25.
Yao, Y., & Sharma, B. (2017). What is in the neighborhood of a tonal syllable? Evidence from auditory lexical decision in Mandarin Chinese. Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America, 21, 1–14.
Yip, M. C. (2002). Access to homophonic meanings during spoken language comprehension: Effects of context and neighborhood density. Proceedings of the International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, 1665–1668.
Zhang, S., Perfetti, C. A., & Yang, H. (1999). Whole word, frequency-general phonology in semantic processing of Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 251, 858–875.
Cited by (7)
Cited by seven other publications
Bailey, Lyam M., Kate Lockary & Eve Higby
2024. Cross-linguistic influence in the bilingual lexicon: Evidence for ubiquitous facilitation and context-dependent interference effects on lexical processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 27:3 ► pp. 495 ff.
Botezatu, Mona Roxana & Dalia L. Garcia
2024. Inhibitory and facilitative effects of lexical neighbors in spoken word recognition: The role of language experience. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 46:2 ► pp. 550 ff.
Botezatu, Mona Roxana, Maya Misra & Judith F. Kroll
Casado, Alba, Jonas Walther, Agata Wolna, Jakub Szewczyk, Antonella Sorace & Zofia Wodniecka
2023. Advantages of visiting your home country: how brief reimmersion in their native country impacts migrants’ native language access. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 26:5 ► pp. 1026 ff.
Yenkimaleki, Mahmood & Vincent J. van Heuven
2023. Effect of pedagogic intervention in enhancing speech fluency by EFL students: A longitudinal study. Language Teaching Research
Bajo, M. Teresa, Carlos J. Gómez-Ariza & Alejandra Marful
2021. Inhibitory Control of Information in Memory Across Domains. Current Directions in Psychological Science 30:5 ► pp. 444 ff.
Salig, Lauren K., Jorge R. Valdés Kroff, L. Robert Slevc & Jared M. Novick
2021. Moving From Bilingual Traits to States: Understanding Cognition and Language Processing Through Moment-to-Moment Variation. Neurobiology of Language 2:4 ► pp. 487 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.