References (10)
References
Hackman, D., & Farah, M. (2008). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 131, 65–73. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hackman, D., Farah, M., & Meaney, M. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 111, 651–659. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Heckman. J.J. (2006). Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science, 312(5782), 1900–1902. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. (2002). Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45(3), 337–374. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Neville, H., Stevens, C., Klein, S., Fanning, J., Bell, T., Cakir, E., & Pakulak, E. (2011). Improving behavior, cognition and neural mechanisms of attention in at-risk children. Paper presented at the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco.
Pakulak, E., & Neville, H. (2010). Proficiency differences in syntactic processing of monolingual native speakers indexed by event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 2728–2529. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011). Maturational constraints on the recruitment of early processes for syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 2752–2765. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raizada, R.D. S., Richards, T.L., Meltzoff, A., & Kuhl, P.K. (2008). Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialization of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children. NeuroImage, 401, 1392–1401. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stevens, C., Lauinger, B., & Neville, H. (2009). Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: An event-related brain potential study. Developmental Science, 12(4), 634–646. DOI logoGoogle Scholar