Article published In:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 12:6 (2022) ► pp.845871
References (73)
References
Abu-Akel, A., Bailey, A. L., & Thum, Y.-M. (2004). Describing the acquisition of determiners in English: A growth modeling approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 33(5), 407–424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alarcón, I. (2011). Spanish gender agreement under complete and incomplete acquisition: Early and late bilinguals’ linguistic behavior within the noun phrase. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 332–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Albirini, A., Benmamoun, E., & Chakrani, B. (2013). Gender and number agreement in the oral production of Arabic HSs. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(01), 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Albirini, A., Benmamoun, E., & Saadah, E. (2011). Grammatical features of Egyptian and Palestinian Arabic HSs’ oral production. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(2), 273–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bassano, D., Maillochon, I., & Mottet, S. (2008). Noun grammaticalization and determiner use in French children’s speech: A gradual development with prosodic and lexical influences. Journal of Child Language, 351, 403–438. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. C. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia and real-time processing. Language and Cognitive Processes. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bayram, F., Rothman, J., Iverson, M., Kupisch, T., Miller, D., Puig-Mayenco, E., & Westergaard, M. (2017). Differences in use without deficiencies in competence: passives in the Turkish and German of Turkish HSs in Germany. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 22(8), 919–939. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bayram, F., Rothman, J., Pisa, G., & Slabakova, R. (2020). Current trends and emerging methodologies in charting heritage language bilingual grammars. Psyarxiv. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1994). Representation and ways of knowing: Three issues in second language acquisition. In N. C. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 549–569). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bolonyai, A. (2007). (In)vulnerable agreement in incomplete bilingual L1 learners. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11(1), 3–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bowles, M. A. (2011). Measuring implicit and explicit linguistic knowledge: What can heritage language learners contribute? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 331, 247–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bull, W. (1965). Spanish for teachers: Applied linguistics. New York: Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Carroll, S. (1989). Second-language acquisition and the computational paradigm. Language Learning, 39(4), 535–594. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. (1986). Acquisition of Romance, with special reference to French. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition: the data (Vol. 1) (pp. 687–782). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Corbett, G. (1991). Gender. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E., Valdés Kroff, J. R., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., & Gerfen, C. (2013). When gender and looking go hand in hand. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(2), 353–387. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eichler, N., Jansen, V., & Müller, N. (2013). Gender acquisition in bilingual children: French–German, Italian–German, Spanish–German and Italian–French. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17(5), 550–572. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, Z., Polinsky, M., & Scontras, G. (2015). The differential representation of number and gender in Spanish. Linguistic Review, 32(4), 703–737. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Godson, L. (2003). Phonetics of language attrition: Vowel production and articulatory setting in the speech of Western Armenian HSs. University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2012). Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem? Second Language Research, 28(2), 191–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Håkansson, G. (1995). Syntax and morphology in language attrition: a study of five bilingual expatriate Swedes. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 5(2), 153–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. W. (1991). The Exponence of Gender in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry, 22(1), 27–62.Google Scholar
Hernandez Pina, F. (1984). Teorías psicosociolingüísticas y su aplicación a la adquisición del español como lengua materna. Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Hockett, C. (1958). A course in modern linguistics. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logo
Hopp, H. (2013). Grammatical gender in adult L2 acquisition: Relations between lexical and syntactic variability. Second Language Research, 29(1), 33–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H., & Lemmerth, N. (2016). Lexical and syntactic congruency in L2 predictive gender processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(1), 171–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaan, E. (2014). Predictive sentence processing in L2 and L1. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4(2), 257–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kang, H.-S., & Kim, I.-S. (2012). Perceived and actual competence and ethnic identity in heritage language learning: a case of Korean-American college students. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 15(3), 279–294. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). A functional approach to child language: A study of determiners and reference (Cambridge Studies in Linguistics 24). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kramer, R. (2015). The Morphosyntax of Gender. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuchenbrandt, I. (2005). Gender acquisition in bilingual Spanish. Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on …, (April), 1252–1263. Retrieved from [URL]
Kupisch, T., Müller, N., & Cantone, K. F. (2002). Gender in monolingual and bilingual first language acquisition: Comparing Italian and French. Lingue e Linguaggio, 1(1), 107–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T., Akpinar, D., & Stöhr, A. (2013). Gender assignment and gender agreement in adult bilinguals and second language learners of French. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3(2), 150–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2007). Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 18(3), 193–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010). Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 63(4), 447–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C. (1998). Proto-articles in the acquisition of Spanish: Interface between phonology and morphology. In R. Fabri, A. Ortmann, & T. Parodi (Eds.), Modelle der Flexion: 18. Jahrestagung der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Sprachwissenschaft (pp. 175–195). Niemeyer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997). Filler syllables, proto-articles and early prosodic constraints in Spanish and German. Proceedings of GALA ’97 Conference on Language Acquisition, 251–256. University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Loerts, H., Wieling, M., & Schmid, M. S. (2013). Neuter is not common in Dutch: Eye movements reveal asymmetrical gender processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 421, 551–570. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
López-Ornat, S. (1997). What lies in between a pre-grammatical and a grammatical representation? Evidence on nominal and verbal form-function mappings in Spanish from 1;7 to 2;1. In A. T. Perez-Leroux & W. R. Glass (Eds.), Contemporary Perspectives on the Acquisition of Spanish, Vol 1: Developing Grammars (pp. 3–20). Cascadilla.Google Scholar
López-Ornat, S., Fernández, A., Gallo, P., & Mariscal, S. (Eds.). (1994). La adquisicion de la lengua espanola. Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1978). The acquisition of morphophonology. Monographcs of the Society for Research on Child Development, 431, 1–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The language experience and proficiency questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940–967. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mariscal, S. (2009). Early acquisition of gender agreement in the Spanish noun phrase: starting small. Journal of Child Language, 361, 143–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996). Adquisiciones morfosintacticas en torno al sintagma nominal: El genero gramatical en espanol. In M. Pérez-Pereira (Ed.), Estudios sobre la adquisicion del Castellano, Catalan, Euskera y Gallego (pp. 263–272). Universidade de Santiago de Compostela, Servicio de Publicaciones.Google Scholar
(2001). Es ‘a pie’ equivalente a DET + N? Sobre el conocimiento temprano de las categorias gramaticales. Cognitiva, 13(1), 35–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2016). The Acquisition of Heritage Languages. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Bhatt, R. M., & Bhatia, A. (2012). Erosion of case and agreement in Hindi HSs. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 2(2), 141–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Davidson, J., de la Fuente, I., & Foote, R. (2014). Early language experience facilitates the processing of gender agreement in Spanish HSs. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(01), 118–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., de la Fuente, I., Davidson, J., & Foote, R. (2013). The role of experience in the acquisition and production of diminutives and gender in Spanish: Evidence from L2 learners and HSs. Second Language Research, 29(1), 87–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R., & Perpiñán, S. (2008). Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish HSs: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning, 58(3), 503–553. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Potowski, K. (2007). Command of gender agreement in school-age Spanish-English Bilingual Children. The International Journal of Bilingualism ’International Journal of Bilingualism, 11(3), 301–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mueller Gathercole, V. C. (2002). Grammatical gender in bilingual and monolingual children: A Spanish morphosyntactic distinction. In R. Oller & R. Eilers (Eds.), Language and Literacy in Bilingual Children (pp. 207–219). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Müller, N. (1987). Der Genuserwerb im Französischen und Deutschen: Eine empirische Untersuchung eines bilingualen Kindes (unpublished master’s thesis). University of Hamburg.Google Scholar
Paris, G., Weber, A., & Crocker, M. W. (2006). No Title. Poster Presented at the 12th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP 2006). Nijmegen, Netherlands.Google Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M. (1991). The acquisition of gender: What Spanish children tell us. Journal of Child Language, 18(3), 571–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piantadosi, S. T., Tily, H., & Gibson, E. (2018). Information content versus word length in natural language: A reply to Ferrer-i-Cancho and Moscoso del Prado Martin (2011).Google Scholar
Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. V. M. (1997). Slot and frame patterns and the development of the determiner category. In Applied Psycholinguists (Vol. 181). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2006). Incomplete acquisition: American Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics, 14(2), 191–262.Google Scholar
(2008). Russian gender under incomplete acquisition. The Heritage Language Journal, 6(1). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018). Heritage Languages and Their Speakers. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1997). Cross-linguistic parallels in language loss. Southwest Journal of Linguistics, 14(1–2), 87–123.Google Scholar
Prado, M. (1982). El genero en español y la teoria de la marcadez. Source: Hispania, 65(2), 258–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roca, I. M. (1989). The organization of grammatical gender. Transactions of the Philological Society, 871, 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J. (2007). HS competence differences, language change, and input type: Inflected infinitives in Heritage Brazilian Portuguese. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11(4), 359–389. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanchez-Sadek, C., Kiraithe, J., & Villarreal, H. (1975). The Acquisition of the Concept of Grammatical Gender in Monolingual and Bilingual Speakers of Spanish.Google Scholar
Scontras, G., Polinsky, M., & Fuchs, Z. (2018). In support of representational economy: Agreement in heritage Spanish. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 3(1), 1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snyder, W., Senghas, A., & Inman, K. (2001). Agreement Morphology and the Acquisition of Noun-Drop in Spanish. Language Acquisition, 9(2), 157–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soler, R. (1984). Acquisicion y utilizacion del articulo. In M. Siguan (Ed.), Estudios sobre psicologia del lenguaje infantil. Piramide.Google Scholar
Thal, D. J., Bates, E., Goodman, J., & Jahn-Samilo, J. (1997). Continuity of language abilities: An exploratory study of late- and early-talking toddlers. Developmental Neuropsychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ticio Quesada, E. (2018). The emergence of nominal expressions in Spanish-English early bilinguals: Economy and bilingual first language acquisition. Studies in bilingualism, v. 561. John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). The item-based nature of children’s early syntactic development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 156–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
William, E. B. (1955). Spanish and English Dictionary. Henry Holt & Co.Google Scholar
Cited by (15)

Cited by 15 other publications

Bosch, Jasmijn E. & Francesca Foppolo
2024. Prediction during spoken language processing in monolingual and multilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 14:4  pp. 512 ff. DOI logo
Fuchs, Zuzanna, Maria Polinsky & Gregory Scontras
2024. Chapter 12. Explaining gender. In Multifaceted Multilingualism [Studies in Bilingualism, 66],  pp. 292 ff. DOI logo
Ito, Aine, Huong Thi Thu Nguyen & Pia Knoeferle
2024. German-dominant Vietnamese heritage speakers use semantic constraints of German for anticipation during comprehension in Vietnamese. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 27:1  pp. 57 ff. DOI logo
Karaca, Figen, Susanne Brouwer, Sharon Unsworth & Falk Huettig
2024. Morphosyntactic predictive processing in adult heritage speakers: effects of cue availability and spoken and written language experience. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 39:1  pp. 118 ff. DOI logo
Montrul, Silvina, Sara Ann Mason & Andrew Armstrong
2024. The Role of Language Experience in the Acquisition of Spanish Gender Agreement: A Study with Nonce Nouns. Languages 9:2  pp. 45 ff. DOI logo
Parshina, Olga, Nina Ladinskaya, Lidia Gault & Irina A. Sekerina
2024. Predictive Language Processing in Russian Heritage Speakers: Task Effects on Morphosyntactic Prediction in Reading. Languages 9:5  pp. 158 ff. DOI logo
Gharibi, Khadij, Fatih Bayram & Gustavo Guajardo
Jegerski, Jill & Gregory D. Keating
2023. Using self-paced reading in research with heritage speakers: a role for reading skill in the online processing of Spanish verb argument specifications. Frontiers in Psychology 14 DOI logo
Luque, Alicia, Eleonora Rossi, Maki Kubota, Megan Nakamura, César Rosales, Cristina López-Rojas, Yulia Rodina & Jason Rothman
2023. Morphological transparency and markedness matter in heritage speaker gender processing: an EEG study. Frontiers in Psychology 14 DOI logo
Sagarra, Nuria & Joseph V. Casillas
2023. Practice beats age: co-activation shapes heritage speakers’ lexical access more than age of onset. Frontiers in Psychology 14 DOI logo
Fuchs, Zuzanna
2022. Eyetracking evidence for heritage speakers’ access to abstract syntactic agreement features in real-time processing. Frontiers in Psychology 13 DOI logo
Fuchs, Zuzanna
2024. Processing of grammatical gender agreement morphemes in Polish: evidence from the Visual World Paradigm. Morphology 34:1  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Keating, Gregory D.
2022. The Effect of Age of Onset of Bilingualism on Gender Agreement Processing in Spanish as a Heritage Language. Language Learning 72:4  pp. 1170 ff. DOI logo
Keating, Gregory D.
2024. Morphological Markedness and the Temporal Dynamics of Gender Agreement Processing in Spanish as a Majority and a Heritage Language. Language Learning DOI logo
Parshina, Olga, Anastasiya Lopukhina & Irina A. Sekerina
2022. Can Heritage Speakers Predict Lexical and Morphosyntactic Information in Reading?. Languages 7:1  pp. 60 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 16 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.