Article published In:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 13:2 (2023) ► pp.218237
References (45)
References
Bauer, R. S., & Benedict, P. K. (1997). Modern Cantonese phonology. Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bhatara, A., Yeung, H. H., & Nazzi, T. (2015). Foreign language learning in french speakers is associated with rhythm perception, but not with melody perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(2), 277–282.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H. (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10(3), 89–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bidelman, G., Gandour, J., & Krishnan, A. (2011). Cross-domain effects of music and language experience on the representation of pitch in the human auditory brainstem. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 231, 425–434. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bidelman, G., Hutka, S., & Moreno, S. (2013). Tone language speakers and musicians share enhanced perceptual and cognitive abilities for musical pitch: Evidence for bidirectionality between the domains of language and music. Plos One, 81, e60676. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bradley, E. D. (2016). Phonetic dimensions of tone language effects on musical melody perception. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 26(4), 337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bradlow, A. R., Akahane-Yamada, R., Pisoni, D. B., & Tohkura, Y. (1999). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/and /l/: Long-term retention of learning in perception and production. Perception & Psychophysics, 61(5), 977–985. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Callan, A. M., Callan, D. E., Tajima, K., Kubo, R., Masaki, S., & Akahane-Yamada, R. (2003). Learning-induced neural plasticity associated with improved identification performance after training of a difficult second-language phonetic contrast. Neuroimage, 19(1), 113–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, A., Liu, L., & Kager, R. (2016). Cross-domain correlation in pitch perception, the influence of native language. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(6), 751–760. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, A., Peter, V., Wijnen, F., Schnack, H., & Burnham, D. (2018). Are lexical tones musical? native language’s influence on neural response to pitch in different domains. Brain and Language, 180–1821, 31–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Delogu, F., Lampis, G., & Belardinelli, M. O. (2006). Music-to-language transfer effect: May melodic ability improve learning of tonal languages by native nontonal speakers? Cognitive Processing, 7(3), 203–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., Marvin, E., & Xu, H. (2006). Absolute pitch among American and Chinese conservatory students: Prevalence differences, and evidence for a speech-related critical period. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 119(2), 719–722. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., Bohn, O.-S., & Jang, S. (1997). Effects of experience on non-native speakers’ production and perception of English vowels. Journal of Phonetics, 25(4), 437–470. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gandour, J., & Harshman, R. (1978). Cross-language difference in tone perception: A multidimensional scaling investigation. Language and Speech, 211, 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gandour, J. (1983). Tone perception in far eastern languages. Journal of Phonetics, 111, 149–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garbin, G., Costa, A., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Rodriguez-Pujadas, A., Ventura, N., … Ávila, C. (2011). Neural bases of language switching in high and early proficient bilinguals. Brain and Language, 119(3), 129–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kempe, V., Bublitz, D., & Brooks, P. J. (2015). Musical ability and non-native speech-sound processing are linked through sensitivity to pitch and spectral information. British Journal of Psychology, 106(2), 349–366. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kraus, N., & Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). Music training for the development of auditory skills. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(8), 599–605. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krizman, J., Marian, V., Shook, A., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2012). Subcortical encoding of sound is enhanced in bilinguals and relates to executive function advantages. Proceedings of National Academy of Science USA, 109(20), 7877. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logo
Lee, C. Y., & Hung, T. H. (2008). Identification of mandarin tones by English-speaking musicians and nonmusicians. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1241, 3235–3248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levi, S. V. (2005). Acoustic correlates of lexical accent in Turkish. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 35(1), 73–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, C., & Thompson, S. A. (1989). Mandarin Chinese: A functional reference grammar. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Lively, S. E., Pisoni, D. B., Yamada, R. A., Tohkura, Y., & Yamada, T. (1994). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/. III. long-term retention of new phonetic categories. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 96(4), 2076–2087. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Margarita, K. (2007). The language experience and proficiency questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940–967. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marie, C., Delogu, F., Lampis, G., Belardinelli, M., & Besson, M. (2011). Influence of musical expertise on segmental and tonal processing in mandarin Chinese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 231, 2701–2715. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U., O’Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Price, C. J. (2004). Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature, 431(7010), 757. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Milovanov, R., Huotilainen, M., Välimäki, V., Esquef, P. A. A., & Tervaniemi, M. (2008). Musical aptitude and second language pronunciation skills in school-aged children: Neural and behavioral evidence. Brain Research, 11941, 81–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mok, P. K. P., & Zuo, D. (2012). The separation between music and speech: Evidence from the perception of cantonese tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132(4), 2711–2720. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moore, C. B., & Jongman, A. (1997). Speaker normalization in the perception of mandarin chinese tones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102(3), 1864–1877. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logo
Osterhout, L., Poliakov, A., Inoue, K., McLaughlin, J., Valentine, G., Pitkanen, I., … Hirschensohn, J. (2008). Second-language learning and changes in the brain. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(6), 509–521. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 691, 265–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfordresher, P., & Brown, S. (2009). Enhanced production and perception of musical pitch in tone language speakers. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 71(-6), 1385–1398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J. B. (1980). The phonology and phonetics of English intonation. [Doctoral dissertation]. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Roncaglia-Denissen, M., Bouwer, F. L., & Honing, H. (2018). Decision making strategy and the simultaneous processing of syntactic dependencies in language and music. Frontiers in Psychology, 91, 38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roncaglia-Denissen, M., Roor, D., Chen, A., & Sadakata, M. (2016). The enhanced musical rhythmic perception in second language learners. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 101, 288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roncaglia-Denissen, M., Schmidt-Kassow, M., Heine, A., Vuust, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2013). Enhanced musical rhythmic perception in turkish early and late learners of german. Frontiers in Psychology, 41, 645. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roncaglia-Denissen, M., Schmidt-Kassow, M., & Kotz, S. A. (2013). Speech rhythm facilitates syntactic ambiguity resolution: ERP evidence. Plos One, 8(2), e56000. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (2009). When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging, 30(4), 507–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schellenberg, E. G. (2015). Music training and speech perception: A gene-environment interaction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 13371, 170–177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schlegel, A. A., Rudelson, J. J., & Tse, P. U. (2012). White matter structure changes as adults learn a second language. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(8), 1664–1670. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shaefer, V., & Darcy, I. (2014). Lexical function of pitch in the first language shapes cross-linguistic perception of Thai tones. Laboratory Phonology, 51, 489–522. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slevc, L. R., & Miyake, A. (2006). Individual differences in second-language proficiency: Does musical ability matter? Psychological Science, 17(8), 675–681. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wallentin, M., Nielsen, A. H., Friis-Olivarius, M., Vuust, C., & Vuust, P. (2010). The musical ear test, a new reliable test for measuring musical competence. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(3), 188–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wong, C. M. P., Skoe, E., Russo, N. M., Dees, T., & Kraus, N. (2007). Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding of linguistic pitch patterns. Nature Neuroscience, 101, 420–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (2)

Cited by two other publications

Sun, Bo, Juan Yang & Zhijie Liang
2024. How music facilitates second language (L2) learning: a systematic review from 2002 to 2022. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.