Article published In:
Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism
Vol. 4:1 (2014) ► pp.133
References (81)
References
Alarcón, I.V. (2011). Spanish gender agreement under complete and incomplete acquisition: Early and late bilinguals’ behavior within the noun phrase. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 332–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alexiadou, A., Anagnostopoulou, E., & Everaert, M (Eds.). (2004). The unaccusativity puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, B., Pine, J., Rowland, C., & Young, C. (2008). The effect of verb semantic class and verb frequency (entrenchment) on children’s and adults’ graded judgments of argument-structure overgeneralization errors. Cognition, 1061, 87–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aranda, A. (1990). La expresión de la causatividad en español actual [Causativity expression in contemporary Spanish]. Zaragoza: Libros Pórticos.Google Scholar
Au, T., Knightly, L., Jun, S., & Oh, J. (2002). Overhearing a language during childhood. Psychological Science, 131, 238–243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beaudrie, S., & Ducar, C. (2005). Beginning level university heritage programs: Creating a space for all heritage language learners. Heritage Language Journal, 31, 1–26.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1982). Evaluating competing linguistic models with language acquisition data: Implications of developmental errors with causative verbs. Quaderni di Semantica, 31, 5–66.Google Scholar
. (1988). The “no negative evidence” problem: How do children avoid constructing an overly general grammar? In J.A. Hawkins (Ed.), Explaining language universals (pp. 73–101). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Braine, M., & Brooks, P.J. (1995). Verb argument structure and the problem of avoiding an overgeneral grammar. In M. Tomasello & W.E. Merriman (Eds.), Beyond names for things: Young children’s acquisition of verbs (pp. 352–376). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brooks, P.J., Tomasello, M., Dodson, K., & Lewis, L. (1999). Young children’s overgeneralizations with fixed transitivity verbs. Child Development, 701, 1325–1337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burzio, L. (1986). Italian syntax: A government-binding approach. Dordrecht: Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cabrera, M., & Zubizarreta, M.L. (2003). On the acquisition of Spanish causative structures by L1 speakers of English. In J. Liceras (Ed.), Proceedings of the 6th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (pp. 24–33). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
. (2005). Overgeneralization of causatives and transfer in L2 Spanish and L2 English. In D. Eddington (Ed.), Selected Proceedings of the 6th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages (pp. 15–30). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Cabrera, M. & Zubizarreta, M.L. (2007). Transfer in periphrastic causatives in L2 English and L2 Spanish. In S. Baauw, F. Drijkoningen, & M. Pinto (Eds.), Romance languages and linguistic theory 2005 (pp. 39–58). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carreira, M., & Potowski, K. (2011). Commentary: Pedagogical implications of experimental SNS research. Heritage Language Journal, 81, 134–151.Google Scholar
Chierchia, G. (1989). A semantics for unaccusatives and its syntactic consequences. Unpublished manuscript, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY.
Cuza, A., & Frank, J. (2011). Transfer effects at the syntax-semantics interface: The case of double-que questions in heritage Spanish. The Heritage Language Journal, 81, 66–89.Google Scholar
Davies, M. (2006). A frequency dictionary of Spanish: Core vocabulary for learners. New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Prada Pérez, A., & Pascual y Cabo, D. (2011). Invariable gusta in the Spanish of heritage speakers in the US. In J. Hershenschon & D. Tanner (Eds.), Proceedings of the 11th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference (pp. 110–120). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Dixon, R.M.W. (2000). A typology of causatives: Form, syntax, and meaning. In R.M.W. Dixon & A.Y. Aikhenvald (Eds.), Changing valency: Case studies in transitivity (pp. 30–83). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fairclough, M., Belpoliti, F., & Bermejo, E. (2010). Developing an electronic placement examination for heritage learners of Spanish: Challenges and payoffs. Hispania, 931, 273–291.Google Scholar
Gropen, J., Blaskovich, J., & DeDe, G. (1996). Come it closer: Causative errors in child speech. In A. Stringfellow, D. Cahana-Armita, E. Hughes, & A. Zukowski (Eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 272–283). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Helms-Park, R. (2001). Evidence of lexical transfer in learner syntax: The acquisition of English causatives by speakers of Hindi-Urdu and Vietnamese. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 231, 71–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopper, P., & Thompson, S. (1980). Transitivity in grammar and discourse. Language, 561, 251–299.Google Scholar
Juffs, A. (1996). Learnability and the lexicon. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Legendre, G. (2007). On the typology of auxiliary selection. Lingua, 117(9), 1522–1540. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (1995). Unaccusativity: At the syntax-lexical semantics interface. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
. (2005). Argument realization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lynch, A. (2008). The linguistic similarities of Spanish heritage and second language learners. Foreign Language Annals, 411, 252–281. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Longman Dictionary of Contemporary English (5th ed.). (2009). London: Pearson Longman.Google Scholar
Marcotte, J.P. (2005). Causative alternation errors in child language acquisition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, Stanford, CA.
. (2006). Causative alternation errors as event-driven construction paradigm completions. In E.V. Clark & B.F Kelly (Eds.), Constructions in acquisition (pp. 1–23). Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
McKoon, G., & Macfarland, T. (2000). Externally and internally caused change of state verbs. Language, 761, 833–858. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mirabal, D. (2009). Vivas en su jardín: La verdadera historia de las hermanas Mirabal y su lucha por la libertad [Alive in their garden: The true story of the Mirabal sisters and their fight for freedom]. New York: Vintage Español.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. (1999). Causative errors with unaccusative verbs in L2 Spanish. Second Language Research, 151, 191–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2001). Agentive verbs of manner of motion in Spanish and English as second languages. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 231, 171–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2005). Second language acquisition and first language loss in adult early bilinguals: Exploring some differences and similarities. Second Language Research, 21(3), 199–249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2006). On the bilingual competence of Spanish heritage speakers: Syntax, lexical-semantics and processing. International Journal of Bilingualism, 101, 37–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2008). Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism: Re-examining the age factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2009). Knowledge of tense-aspect and mood in Spanish heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 131, 239–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2010). Dominant language transfer in adult second language learners and heritage speakers. Second Language Research, 26(3), 293–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2011a). Spanish heritage speakers: Bridging formal linguistics, psycholinguistics, and pedagogy [Introduction to special issue]. Heritage Language Journal, 81, i–v.Google Scholar
. (2011b). Morphological errors in Spanish second language learners and heritage speakers. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 331, 163–192.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., & Bowles, M. (2009). Back to basics: Incomplete knowledge of differential object marking in Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 121, 363–383. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R., & Perpiñán, S. (2008). Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning, 581, 503–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Ionin, T. (2010). Transfer effects in the interpretation of definite articles by Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 131, 449–473. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Polinsky, M. (2011). Why not heritage speakers? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11, 58–62.Google Scholar
Moore, M. (1993). Second language acquisition of lexically constrained transitivity alternations: Acquisition of the causative alternation by second language learners of English. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC.
Obregon, P. (2010). Usage and experiential factors as predictors of Spanish morphosyntactic competence in US heritage speakers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH.
Otheguy, R., & Stern, N. (2011). On so-called Spanglish. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15(1), 85–100. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pascual y Cabo, D., & Rothman, J. (2012). The (il) logical problem of heritage speaker bilingualism and incomplete acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 33(4), 450–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perlmutter, D.M. (1978). Impersonal passives and the unaccusative hypothesis. In Proceedings of the Fourth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 157–189). University of California, Berkeley. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pires, A., & Rothman, J. (2009). Disentangling sources of incomplete acquisition: An explanation for competence divergence across heritage grammars. International Journal of Bilingualism, 131, 211–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2008). Gender under incomplete acquisition: Heritage speakers’ knowledge of noun categorization. Heritage Language Journal, 61, 40–71.Google Scholar
. (2011). Reanalysis in adult heritage language: New evidence in support of attrition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 331, 305–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Potowski, K., Jegerski, J., & Morgan-Short, K. (2009). The effects of instruction on linguistic development in Spanish heritage language speakers. Language Learning, 591, 537–579. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pye, C. 1994: A cross-linguistic approach to the causative alternation. In Y. Levi (Ed.), Other children, other languages (pp. 109–138). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Real Academia Española. (2005). Diccionario panhispánico de dudas. Spain: Santillana.Google Scholar
Roggia, A. (2011). Unaccusativity and word order in Mexican Spanish: An examination of syntactic interfaces and the split intransitivity hierarchy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA.
Rothman, J. (2007). Heritage speaker competence differences, language change and input type: Inflected infinitives in heritage Brazilian Portuguese. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11(4), 359–389. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2009a). Understanding the nature and outcomes of early bilingualism: Romance languages as heritage languages. International Journal of Bilingualism, 131, 155–163.Google Scholar
. (2009b). Pragmatic deficits with syntactic consequences?: L2 pronominal subjects and the syntax-pragmatics interface. Journal of Pragmatics, 411, 951–973.Google Scholar
Ruiz-Sánchez, C. (2006). Causative hacer and dejar . In J.C. Clements & J. Yoon (Eds.), Functional approaches to Spanish syntax (pp. 278–299). UK: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shibatani, M. (1976). The grammar of causative constructions: A conspectus. In M. Shibatani (Ed.), Syntax and semantics 6: The grammar of causative constructions (pp. 1–40). New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shibatani, M., & Pardeshi, P. (2002). The causative continuum. In M. Shibatani (Ed.), The grammar of causation and interpersonal manipulation (pp. 85–126). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (2008). The limits of convergence in language contact. Journal of Language Contact, 21, 213–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, C.S. (1970). Jespersen’s ‘move and change’ class and causative verbs in English. In M.A. Jazayery, E.C. Polomé, & W. Winter (Eds.), Linguistic and literary studies in honor of Archibald A. Hill: Descriptive linguistics (Vol. 21, pp. 101–109). Mouton: The Hague.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. (2000). Gradients in auxiliary selection with intransitive verbs. Language, 76(4), 859–890. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2004). Gradience at the lexicon-syntax interface: Evidence from auxiliary selection and implications for unaccusativity. In A. Alexiadou, E. Anagnostopoulou, & M. Everaert (Eds.), The unaccusativity puzzle (pp. 243–268). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. (2011). Pinning down the concept of ‘interface’ in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 11, 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Theakston, A. (2004). The role of entrenchment in children’s and adults’ performance on grammaticality judgment tasks. Cognitive Development, 191, 15–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I., & Sorace, A. (2006). Differentiating interfaces: L2 performance in syntax-semantics and syntax-discourse phenomena. In D. Bamman, T. Magnitskaia, & C. Zaller (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 653–664). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Valdés, G. (2005). Bilingualism, heritage language learners, and SLA research: Opportunities lost or seized? The Modern Language Journal, 89(3), 410–426. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L. (2011). Second language acquisition at the interfaces. Lingua, 1211, 577–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wolff, P. (2003). Direct causation in the linguistic coding and individuation of causal events. Cognition, 881, 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zapata, G., Sánchez, L., & Toribio, A.J. (2005). Contact and contracting Spanish. International Journal of Bilingualism, 91, 377–395. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zubizarreta, M.L. (1985). The relation between morphophonology and morphosyntax: The case of Romance causatives. Linguistic Inquiry, 161, 247–289.Google Scholar
Zubizarreta, M.L., & Oh, E. (2007). On the syntactic composition of manner and motion. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zyzik, E. (in press). Expressing causation in Spanish: A functional analysis of heritage speakers’ written production. Heritage Language Journal, 11(1).
Cited by (6)

Cited by six other publications

Gonzalez, Becky
2023. Syntactic licensing in heritage Spanish: Psych verbs and the middle voice. International Journal of Bilingualism 27:5  pp. 776 ff. DOI logo
Ionin, Tania
2021. Semantics of Heritage Languages. In The Cambridge Handbook of Heritage Languages and Linguistics,  pp. 668 ff. DOI logo
Gonzalez, Becky Halloran
2020. The Syntactic Distribution of Object Experiencer Psych Verbs in Heritage Spanish. Languages 5:4  pp. 63 ff. DOI logo
Ortega, Lourdes, Sang-Ki Lee & Munehiko Miyata
2018. Chapter 9. What is happened? Your amazon.com order has shipped. In Language Learning, Discourse and Cognition [Human Cognitive Processing, 64],  pp. 213 ff. DOI logo
Ionin, Tania & Eve Zyzik
2014. Judgment and Interpretation Tasks in Second Language Research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics 34  pp. 37 ff. DOI logo
[no author supplied]
2021. Grammatical Aspects of Heritage Languages. In The Cambridge Handbook of Heritage Languages and Linguistics,  pp. 579 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.