Most film viewers know the experience of being deeply absorbed in the story of a popular film. It seems that at such moments they lose awareness of watching a movie. And yet it is highly unlikely that they completely ignore the fact that they watch a narrative and technological construction. Perhaps film viewers experience being in a story world while simultaneously being aware of its construction. Such a dual awareness would seem paradoxical, because the experience of the one would go at the cost of the other. We argue that the solution of this paradox requires dropping the notion of an undivided consciousness, and replacing it with one of consciousness as coming in degrees. In this chapter we present both cognitive and film-analytic arguments for differential awareness of story and narration/technology, and argue that a characteristic of absorption is to be found in story world super-consciousness.
Allen, R. (1993). Representation, illusion, and the cinema. Cinema Journal, 32(2), 21–48.
Ariel, M. (2001). Accessibility theory: An overview. In T. Sanders, J. Schilperoord, & W. Spooren (Eds.), Text representation: Linguistic and psycholinguistic aspects (Vol. 8, pp. 29–87). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Augustin, D., & Leder, H. (2006). Art expertise: A study of concepts and conceptual spaces. Psychology Science, 48(2), 135–156.
Baars, B. J. (1997). In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind. Oxford: OUP.
Baars, B. J. (2002). The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent evidence. Trends in cognitive sciences, 6(1), 47–52.
Bálint, K., & Tan, E. S. H. (2015). “It feels like there are hooks inside my chest”. The construction of narrative absorption experiences using image schemata. Projections, 9, 63–88.
Bezdek, M. A., Gerrig, R. J., Wenzel, W. G., Shin, J., Revill, K. P., & Schumacher, E. H. (2015). Neural evidence that suspense narrows attentional focus. Neuroscience, 303, 338–345.
Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(02), 227–247.
Block, N. (2007). Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(5–6), 481.
Bordwell, D. (1985). Widescreen aesthetics and mise en scene criticism. The Velvet Light Trap, 21, 18. Retrieved from <[URL]>
Bordwell, D. (2002). Intensified continuity visual style in contemporary American film. Film Quarterly, 55(3), 16–28.
Bordwell, D. (2006). The way Hollywood tells it: Story and style in modern movies. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Bordwell, D., Staiger, J., & Thompson, K. (1985). The classical Hollywood cinema: Film style & mode of production to 1960. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.
Bordwell, D., & Thompson, K. (2016). Film art. 11th ed. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Branigan, E. (1992). Narrative comprehension and film. London: Routledge.
Carroll, J. M. (1980). Toward a structural psychology of cinema. The Hague: Mouton.
Carroll, N., & Seeley, W. P. (2013). Cognitivism, psychology and neuroscience: Movies as attentional engines. In A. Shimamura (Ed.), Psychocinematics (pp. 53–75). Oxford: OUP.
Chatman, S. (1978). Story and discourse. New York, NY: Cornell University Press.
Cohn, N. (2013). Visual narrative structure. Cognitive Science, 37(3), 413–452.
Cohn, N., Paczynski, M., Jackendoff, R., Holcomb, P. J., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2012). (Pea) nuts and bolts of visual narrative: Structure and meaning in sequential image comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 65(1), 1–38.
Cutting, J. E. (2016). Narrative theory and the dynamics of popular movies. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 23(6), 1713–1743.
de Gardelle, V., & Kouider, S. (2009). Cognitive theories of consciousness. In W. P. Banks (Ed.), Encyclopedia of consciousness, 1, 135–146. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Dehaene, S.Changeux, J. -P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J. & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204–211.
Doicaru, M. M. (2016). Gripped by movies. From story-world to artifact absorption. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Amsterdam.
Garsoffky, B., Schwan, S., & Huff, M. (2009). Canonical views of dynamic scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 17–27.
Green, M. C., & Donahue, J. K. (2009). Simulated worlds: Transportation into narratives. In K. D. Markman & W. M. P. Klein (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and mental simulation (pp. 241–256). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Grodal, T. (2009). Embodied visions. Oxford: OUP.
Hasson, U., Nir, Y., Levy, I., Fuhrmann, G., & Malach, R. (2003). Intersubject synchronization of cortical vision during natural vision. Science, 303, 1634–1640.
Hinde, S. J. (2016). Attention while watching movies. The study of attention while engaged in the prolonged activity of watching movies. (Unpublished PhD dissertation). University of Bristol
Humphrey, N. (2006). Seeing red. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.
Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2001). Computational modeling of visual attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(3), 194–203.
Jiang, Y., Costello, P., Fang, F., Huang, M., & He, S. (2006). A gender-and sexual orientation-dependent spatial attentional effect of invisible images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(45), 17048–17052.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice & Hall.
Lambie, J. A., & Marcel, A. J. (2002). Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: a theoretical framework. Psychological Review, 109(2), 219–259.
Lamme, V. A. (2010). How neuroscience will change our view on consciousness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(3), 204–220.
Lewinski, P., Fransen, M. L., & Tan, E. S. (2014). Predicting advertising effectiveness by facial expressions in response to amusing persuasive stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 7(1), 1–14.
Loschky, L. C., Larson, A. M., Magliano, J. P., & Smith, T. J. (2015). What would Jaws do? The tyranny of film and the relationship between gaze and higher-level narrative film comprehension. PloS One 10(11), e0142474.
Magliano, J. P., Dijkstra, K., & Zwaan, R. A. (1996). Generating predictive inferences while viewing a movie. Discourse Processes, 22(3), 199–224.
Mandler, G. (1998). Consciousness and mind as philosophical problems and as psychological issues. In J. Hochberg (Ed.), Perception and cognition at century’s end (pp. 45–65). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Mar, R. A., & Oatley, K. (2008). The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 173–192.
McGovern, K., & Baars, B. J. (2007). Cognitive theories of consciousness. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 177–205). Cambridge: CUP.
Metz, C. (1974). On the impression of reality in the cinema. Film Language: A Semiotics of the Cinema, 3–15. Oxford: OUP.
Michotte van den Berck, A. (1948). Le caractère de ‘réalité’ des projections cinématographiques. Revue Internationale de Filmologie, 1, 249–261. [Translation by A. P. Costall, as The character of ‘reality’ of cinematographic projections. In G. Thines, Costall, A. P., & Butterworth, G (Eds.), 1991, Michotte’s experimental phenomenology of perception (pp. 197–207). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawarence Erlbaum Associates].
Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9(3), 184–211.
Oatley, K. (1999). Why fiction may be twice as true as fact: Fiction as cognitive and emotional simulation. Review of General Psychology 3(2), 101–117.
Oatley, K. (2007). Narrative consciousness. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 375–402). Cambridge: CUP.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.
Radvansky, G. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2014). Event cognition. Oxford University Press.
Raz, G., Jacob, Y., Gonen, T., Winetraub, Y., Flash, T., Soreq, E., & Hendler, T. (2014). Cry for her or cry with her: Context-dependent dissociation of two modes of cinematic empathy reflected in network cohesion dynamics. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 30–38.
Rumelhart, D. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In D. Bobrow & A. Collins (Eds.), Representation and understanding (pp. 211–236). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Schank, R. C. (1979). Interestingness: Controlling inferences. Artificial Intelligence, 12(3), 273–297.
Schwann, S., Garsoffky, B., & Hesse, F. W. (2000). Do film cuts facilitate the perceptual and cognitive organization of activity sequences?Memory & Cognition, 28, 214–223.
Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1997). Change blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(7), 261–267.
Smith, M. (2012). Triangulating aesthetic experience. In A. P. Shimamura & S. E. Palmer (Eds.), Aesthetic science: Connecting minds, brains, and experience, (pp. 80–106). Oxford: OUP.
Smith, T. J. (2012). The attentional theory of cinematic continuity. Projections, 6(1), 1–27.
Smith, T. J., & Henderson, J. M. (2008). Edit blindness: The relationship between attention and global change blindness in dynamic scenes. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 2(2) 1–17.
Smith, T. J., Levin, D. T. & Cutting, J. (2012). A window on reality: Perceiving edited moving images. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 101–106.
Suckfüll, M., & Scharkow, M. (2009). Modes of reception for fictional films. Communications, 34(4), 361–384.
Tan, E. S. H. (1996). Emotion and the structure of narrative film. Film as an emotion machine. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Tan, E. S. H. (1995). Film induced affect as a witness emotion. Poetics, 23, 7–32.
Thompson, K. (1999). Storytelling in the new Hollywood: understanding classical narrative technique. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Visch, V., & Tan, E. S. H. (2009). Categorizing moving objects into film genres: The effect of animacy attribution, emotional response, and the deviation from non-fiction. Cognition, 110, 265–272.
Visch, V. T., Tan, E. S. H., & Saakes, D. P. (2015). Viewer knowledge: Application of exposure-based layperson knowledge in genre-specific animation production. International Journal of Design, 9(1), 83–89.
Winston, A., & Cupchik, G. (1992). The evaluation of high art and popular art by naive and experienced viewers. Visual Arts Research, 18(1), 1–14.
Zacks, J. M. (2013). Constructing event representations during film comprehension. In A. P. Shimamura (Ed.), Psychocinematics (pp. 227–243). Oxford: OUP.
Zillmann, D. (2005). Cinematic creation of emotion. In J. D. Anderson & B. F. Fisher Anderson (Eds.), Moving image theory (pp. 164–179). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.
Zwaan, R. A. (2004). The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 35–62.
Cited by (6)
Cited by six other publications
Gulyas, Napsugar, Miruna Doicaru, Wilco Boode, Fabio Campos & Marnix S. van Gisbergen
2024. ‘What Are They Looking at?’ Testing the Capacity of Action Units to Direct Attention in a 360° Recorded Virtual Reality Narrative. In XR and Metaverse [Springer Proceedings in Business and Economics, ], ► pp. 140 ff.
Ready, Jonathan L.
2023. Immersion, Identification, and the Iliad,
Cutting, James E.
2021. Movies on Our Minds,
Polvinen, Merja, Howard Sklar & Anezka Kuzmicova
2019. Mimetic and synthetic views of characters: How readers process “people” in fiction. Cogent Arts & Humanities 6:1 ► pp. 1687257 ff.
Lugea, Jane
2018. The year’s work in stylistics 2017. Language and Literature: International Journal of Stylistics 27:4 ► pp. 329 ff.
Tan, Eduard Sioe-Hao & Valentijn Visch
2018. Co-Imagination of Fictional Worlds in Film Viewing. Review of General Psychology 22:2 ► pp. 230 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 october 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.