References (57)
References
Aksu-Koç, A. & Slobin, D. 1985. The acquisition of Turkish. In The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition, D. Slobin (ed.), 839–878. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Allen, S. 1996. Aspects of Argument Structure in Inuktitut [Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 13]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bailey, N., Madden, C. & Krashen, S. 1974. Is there a “natural sequence” in adult second language learning? Language Learning 24: 235–243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beard, R. 1995. Lexeme-morpheme Based Morphology. Albany NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Berman, R. 1993. Marking of verb transitivity by Hebrew-speaking children. Journal of Child Language 20: 642–669. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, J.D. 1980. Relative merits of four methods for scoring cloze tests. Modern Language Journal 64: 311–317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, D. 1996. L2 Acquisition of English Psych Predicates by French and Chinese Native Speakers. PhD dissertation, McGill University, Montréal.
Chierchia, G. 2004. A semantics for unaccusatives and its syntactic consequences. In The Unaccusativity Puzzle: Explorations of the Syntax-Lexicon Interface, A. Alexiadou, E. Anagnostopoulou, & M. Everaert (eds), 22–59. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. Blakhair, L., Schutter, J. S. & Cunnings, I. 2013. The time course of morphological processing in a second language. Second Language Research 29: 7–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. 1997. Beyond explicit rule learning: Automatized second language morphosyntax. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 19: 195–221. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. 2012. On the acquisition of inflectional morphology. Morphology 22: 1–8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dowty, D. 1979. Word Meaning and Montague Grammar. Dordrecht: Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedline, B. 2011. Challenges in the Second Language Acquisition of Derivational Morphology. From Theory to Practice. PhD dissertation, University of Pittsburgh.
Göksel, A. & Kerslake, C. 2005. Turkish: A Comprehensive Grammar. London: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grimshaw, J. 1990. Argument Structure. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haegeman, L. 1985. The Get passive and Burzio’s generalization. Lingua 66: 53–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Halle, M. & Marantz, A. 1993. Distributed morphology. In The View from Building 20. Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger, K. Hale & S. Keyser (eds), 111–176. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Harley, H. & Noyer, R. 2000. Formal versus encyclopedic properties of vocabulary: Evidence from nominalizations. In The Lexicon-encyclopedia Interface, B. Peters (ed.), 349–374. 
Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, M. 1993. More on the typology of inchoative/causative verb alternations. In Causatives and Transitivity [Studies in Lanuage Companion Series 23], B. Comrie & 
M. Polinsky (eds), 87–120. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirakawa, M. 1995. L2 acquisition of English unaccusative constructions. In Proceedings of the 19th Boston University Conference on Language Development, D. MacLaughlin & S. McEwen (eds), 291–302. Sommerville MA: Cascadilla Press.
Hwang, S.H. & Lardiere, D. 2013. Plural marking in L2 Korean: A feature-based approach. Second Language Research 29: 57–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N. 2002. Form-meaning mapping in vocabulary acquisition in a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 24: 617–637. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A. 1996. Learnability and the Lexicon: Theories and Second Language Acquisition Research [Language Acquisition and Language Disorder 12]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kornfilt, J. 1997. Turkish. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. 2009. Some thoughts on a contrastive analysis of features in second language acquisition. Second Language Research 25: 173–227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2007. Ultimate Attainment in Second Language Acquisition: A Case Study. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Lardiere, D. & Schwartz, B.D. 1997. Feature-marking in the L2 development of deverbal compounds. Journal of Linguistics 33: 327–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Legate, A.J. & Yang, C. 2007. Mophosyntactic learning and the development of Tense. Language Acquisition 14: 315–344. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levin, B. 1993. English Verb Classes and Alternations. Chicago IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Levin, B. & Rappaport Hovav, M. 1995. Unaccusativity at the Syntax-semantics Interface. 
Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marantz, A. 1984. On the Nature of Grammatical Relations. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
. 1997. No escape from syntax: Don’t try morphological analysis in the privacy of your own lexicon. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 4: 201–225.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. 1999a. Causative errors with unaccusative verbs in L2 Spanish. Second Language Research 15: 191–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 1999b. Se o no se: Transitive and intransitive verbs in L2 Spanish. Spanish Applied Linguistics 3: 145–194.Google Scholar
. 2000a. Transitivity alternations in second language acquisition: Toward a modular view of transfer. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 22: 229–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2000b. Causative psych verbs in Spanish L2 acquisition. In Spanish Applied Linguistics at the Turn of the Millennium: Papers from the 1999 Conference on the L1 & L2 Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese , R. Leow & C. Sanz (eds), 97–118. Sommerville MA: Cascadilla Press.
. 2001a. First-language-constrained variability in the second-language acquisition of argument-structure-changing morphology with causative verbs. Second Language Research 17: 144–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2001b. Causatives and transitivity in L2 English. Language Learning 51: 51–106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morales, A. 2014. Production and Comprehension of Verb Agreement Morphology in Spanish and English Child L2 Learners: Evidence for the Effects of Morphological Structure. PhD dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Morikawa, H. 1991. Acquisition of causatives in Japanese. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 30: 80–87.Google Scholar
Oshita, H. 2000. What is happened may not be what appears to be happening: a corpus study of ‘passive’ unaccusatives in L2 English. Second Language Research 16: 293–324.Google Scholar
Pesetsky, D. 1995. Zero Syntax. Experiencers and Cascades. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ramchand, G. 2013. Argument structure and argument structure alternations. In The Cambridge Handbook of Generative Syntax, M. den Dikken (ed.), 265–321. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reinhart, T. 2002. The theta system – an overview. Theoretical Linguistics 28: 229–290.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. & Sprouse, R. 1996. L2 cognitive states and the Full Transfer/Full Access Model. Second Language Research 12: 40–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slabakova, R. 2008. Meaning in the Second Language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Toth, P. 1999. Preemption in instructed learners of Spanish as a foreign language: Acquiring a rule for se . Spanish Applied Linguistics 3: 195–246.Google Scholar
. 2000. The interaction of instruction and learner-internal factors in the acquisition of L2 morphosyntax. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 22: 169–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2008. Teacher- and learner-led discourse in task-based grammar instruction: Providing procedural assistance for L2 morphosyntactic development. Language Learning 58: 237–283. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2011. Social and cognitive factors in making teacher-led classroom discourse relevant for second language development. The Modern Language Journal 95: 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Underhill, R. 1976. Turkish Grammar. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Verkuyl, H. 1993. A Theory of Aspectuality. Cambrige: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B. 1996. Input Processing and Grammar Instruction in Second Language Acquisition. Norwood NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Volpe, J. 2005. Japanese Morphology and its Theoretical Consequences. Derivational Morphology in Distributed Morphology. PhD dissertation, State University of New York, Stony Brook.
White, L., Montrul, S., Hirakawa, M., Chen, D., Bruhn-Garavito, J. & Brown, C. 1998. L2 psych verbs and the T/SM restriction: The status of a zero causative morpheme. In Morphology and its Interfaces in Second Language Knowledge [Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 19], M. Beck (ed.), 257–282. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zobl, H. 1989. Canonical typological structure and ergativity in English L2 acquisition. In Linguistic Perspectives on Second Language Acquisition, S. Gass & J. Schachter (eds), 203–221. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Gonzalez, Becky
2023. A lexical semantic approach to the L2 acquisition of Spanish psych verbs. Second Language Research 39:3  pp. 731 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 25 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.