Part of
On the Acquisition of the Syntax of Romance
Edited by Anna Gavarró
[Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 62] 2018
► pp. 169184
References (43)
References
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barac, R., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2016). Behavioral and electrophysiological differences in executive control between monolingual and bilingual children. Child Development, 87(4), 1277–1290.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Peets, K. F., & Moreno, S. (2014). Producing bilinguals through immersion education: Development of metalinguistic awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 177-191.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L. (1970). Language development: Form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1993). Language development from two to three. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Cardinaletti, A., & Guasti, M. T. (1993). Negation in epistemic small clauses. Probus, 5, 39–62.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (1975). Sentence comprehension: A psycholinguistic processing model of verification. Psychological Review, 82, 45–73.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, H. Q. (1925). Eetong xinli zhi yanjiu [Research on child psychology]. Shanghai: The Commercial Press.Google Scholar
Choi, H. (2014). The answering system to yes–no truth-functional questions in Korean–English bilingual children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 43, 267–287.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Chase, W. G. (1972). On the process of comparing sentences against pictures. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 472–517.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ćoso, B., & Bogunović, I. (2017). The role of linguistic cues in bilingual negation processing. International Journal of Bilingualism, 1–16.Google Scholar
Delfitto D. (2018). Negation as a window on the non-sequential nature of language interpretation and processing. In A. Capone, M. Carapezza, & F. Lo Piparo (Eds.), Further advances in pragmatics and philosophy (pp.81–95). Cham: Springer.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc.Google Scholar
Ernst, T. (1995). Negation in Mandarin Chinese. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 13(4), 665–707.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fan, L. (2007). Early child language acquisition of negative words in Mandarin Chinese. Modern Foreign Languages (Quarterly), 30(2), 144–154.Google Scholar
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Science, 20, 1–55.Google Scholar
Hasegawa, M., Carpenter, P. A., & Just, M. A. (2002). An fMRI study of bilingual sentence comprehension and workload. NeuroImage, 15(3), 647–660.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hasson, U., & Glucksberg, S. (2006). Does understanding negation entail affirmation? An examination of negated metaphors. Journal of Pragmatics, 38(7), 1015–1032.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horn, L. (1989). A natural history of negation. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hu, S., Vender, M., Fiorin, G., & Delfitto, D. (2018). Difficulties in comprehending affirmative and negative Sentences: Evidence from Chinese children with reading difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51(2), 181–193.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaup, B., Lüdtke, J., & Zwaan, R. A. (2005). Effects of negation, truth value, and delay on picture recognition after reading affirmative and negative sentences. In B. G. Bara, & L. Barsal (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.1114–1119). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(2006). Processing negated sentences with contradictory predicates: Is a door that is not open mentally closed? Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 1033–1050.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007). The experiential view of language comprehension: How is negation represented? In F. Schmalhofer, & C. A. Perfetti (Eds.), Higher level language processes in the brain: Inference and comprehension processes (pp.255–288). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Madrid, D., & Garcia, E. E. (1981). Development of negation in bilingual Spanish/English and monolingual English speakers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73(5), 624–631.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nieuwland, M., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2008). When the truth is not too hard to handle: An event-related potential study on the pragmatics of negation. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1213–1218.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nordmeyer, A. E., & Frank, M. C. (2014). The role of context in young children’s comprehension of negation. Journal of Memory and Language, 77, 25–39.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. P., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). Should the search for bilingual advantages in executive functioning continue? Cortex, 74, 305–314.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 1–25.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pea, R. (1980). The development of negation in early child language. In D. Olson (Ed.), The social foundations of language and thought (pp.156–186). New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
R Development Core Team (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. <[URL]>Google Scholar
Scappini, M. (2015). On the processing of sentential negation in dyslexic adults: An ERP study (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Verona.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2012). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.Google Scholar
Stella, G., Pizzioli, C., & Tressoldi, P. E. (2000). Peabody - Test di Vocabolario Recettivo. Torino: Omega Edizioni.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3–24.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vender, M., & Delfitto, D. (2010). Towards a pragmatic of negation: The interpretation of negative sentences in developmental dyslexia. Generative Grammar at Geneva, 6, 1–27.Google Scholar
Vender, M. (2017). Disentangling dyslexia: Phonological and processing impairment in developmental dyslexia. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vender, M., Hu, S., Mantione, F., Savazzi, S., Delfitto, D., & Melloni, C. (2018). Inflectional morphology: Evidence for an advantage of bilingualism in dyslexia. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wason, P. C. (1959). The processing of positive and negative information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11(2), 92–107.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1961). Response to affirmative and negative binary statements. British Journal of Psychology, 52(2), 133–142.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1965). The contexts of plausible denial. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4(1), 7–11.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123(2), 162–185.DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., Stanfield, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2002). Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychological Science, 13, 168–171.DOI logoGoogle Scholar