References
Brown, R.
(1973) A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chevrot, J.-P., Nardy, A., & Barbu, S
(2011) Developmental dynamics of SES-related differences in children’s production of obligatory and variable phonological alternations. Language Sciences, 33, 180–191. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.
(1987) The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp.1–33). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Eisel Hendricks, A., Miller, K., & Jackson, C.
(2015) Regularization or probability-matching? Acquisition of inconsistent gender marking in Fering-speaking children. In E. Grillo, K. Jepson, & M. LaMendola (Eds.), Boston University Conference on Language Development 39, Online Proceedings Supplement. Retrieved at < [URL]Google Scholar
(2018) Regularizing unpredictable variation: Evidence from a natural language setting. Language Learning and Development, 14, 1–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foulkes, P., Docherty, G., & Watt, D.
(2005) Phonological variation in child-directed speech. Language, 81, 177–206. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guasti, M. T., & Rizzi, L.
(2002) Agreement and tense as distinct syntactic positions. In G. Cinque (Ed.), Functional structure in DP and IP: The cartography of syntactic structures (pp.167–194). Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Guy, G., & Boyd, S.
(1990) The development of a morphological class. Language Variation and Change, 3, 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Halberda, J.
(2002) The development of a word-learning strategy. Cognition, 87, B23-B24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoekstra, T., & Hyams, N.
(1998) Aspects of root infinitives. Lingua, 106, 81–112. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kovac, C., & Adamson, H. D.
(1980) Variation theory and first language acquisition. In D. Sankoff & H. Cedergren (Eds.), Variation omnibus: Current inquiry into language and linguistics (pp.403–410). Edmonton, Alberta: Linguistics Research.Google Scholar
Labov, W.
(1989) The child as linguistic historian. Language Variation and Change, 1, 85–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lipski, J. M.
(2015) From “more” to “less”: Spanish, Palenquero (Afro-Colombian creole) and gender agreement. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(9), 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lukyanenko, C., & Miller, K
(2016) Learning the plural from variable input: An eye-tracking study. Paper presented at the Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition in North America (GALANA) 7, University of Illinois.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(2000) The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Markman, E., & Wachtel, G.
(1988) Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meaning of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 121–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, K.
(2007) Variable input and the acquisition of plurality in two varieties of Spanish(Unpublished PhD dissertation). Michigan State University.Google Scholar
(2013) Acquisition of variable rules: /s/-lenition in the speech of Chilean Spanish-speaking children and their caregivers. Language Variation and Change, 25, 311–340. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(in preparation) Dominican Republic child-caregiver corpus (DRCC). National Science Foundation BCS-1322763, < [URL]Google Scholar
Miller, K., & Schmitt, C.
(2010) Effects of variable input in the acquisition of plural in two dialects of Spanish. Lingua, 120, 1178–1193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Variable input and the acquisition of plural morphology. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Language Development, 19, 223–261. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(in preparation) Chilean child-caregiver corpus (CCC). National Science Foundation BCS-1061805 < [URL]Google Scholar
Miller, K.
(2015) Children’s production of ain’t In: P. Donaher & S. Katz (eds.) Ain’thology: The History and Life of the Taboo Word Ain’t. Cambridge Scholars Publishing, 96–112. Google Scholar
Montrul, S. & Potowski, K.
(2007) Command of gender agreement in school-age Spanish-English Bilingual Children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11(3), 301–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oetting, J. B., & McDonald, J.
(2001) Nonmainstream dialect use and specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 44, 207–223. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1984) Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Rispoli, M., Hadley, P., & Holt, J.
(2012) Sequence and system in the acquisition of tense and agreement. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 1007–1021. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, J.
(1997) Acquisition of variable rules: A study of (-t,d) deletion in preschool children. Journal of Child Language, 24, 351–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schütze, C.
(2010) The status of nonagreeing don’t and theories of root infinitives. Language Acquisition, 17(4), 235–271. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schütze, C., & Wexler, K.
(1996) Subject case licensing and English root infinitives. In A. Stringfellow, D. Cahana-Amitay, E. Hughes, & A. Zukowski (Eds.), Proceedings of the 20th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp.670–681). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Shin, N.
(2016) Acquiring constraints on morphosyntactic variation: Children’s Spanish subject pronoun expression. Journal of Child Language, 43, 914–947. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Singleton, J., & Newport, E.
(2004) When learners surpass their models: The acquisition of American Sign Language from inconsistent input. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 370–407. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, J., Durham, M., & Fortune, L.
(2007) Mam, my trousers is fa’in doon!: Community, caregiver, and child in the acquisition of variation in a Scottish dialect. Language Variation and Change, 19, 63–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Universal and dialect-specific pathways of acquisition: Caregivers, children, and t/d deletion. Language Variation and Change, 21, 69–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suppes, P.
(1974) The semantics of children’s language. American Psychologist, 29, 103–114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wexler, K.
(1994) Optional infinitives, head movement, and the economy of derivation in child language. In D. Lightfoot & N. Hornstein (Eds.), Verb movement (pp.305–350). Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1998) Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua, 106, 23–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wolfram, W., & Schilling-Estes, N.
(1998) American English. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Yang, C.
(2002) Knowledge and learning in natural language. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Barrière, Isabelle, Blandine Joseph, Katsiaryna Aharodnik, Sarah Kresh, Guetjens Prince Fleurio, Géraldine Legendre & Thierry Nazzi
Lukyanenko, Cynthia & Karen Miller
2023. Agreeing when to disagree: A corpus analysis of variable agreement in caregiver and child English. Language Variation and Change 35:1  pp. 29 ff. DOI logo
Shin, Naomi & Karen Miller
2022. Children’s Acquisition of Morphosyntactic Variation. Language Learning and Development 18:2  pp. 125 ff. DOI logo
Shin, Naomi & Karen Lynn Miller
2024. Children’s Acquisition of Morphosyntactic Variation: A Reply to Commentaries. Language Learning and Development 20:1  pp. 83 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 22 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.