Part of
The Acquisition of Derivational Morphology: A cross-linguistic perspective
Edited by Veronika Mattes, Sabine Sommer-Lolei, Katharina Korecky-Kröll and Wolfgang U. Dressler
[Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 66] 2021
► pp. 120
References (75)
References
Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C. F. & Theakston, A. 2015. The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 42(2): 239–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anglin, J. M. 1993. Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 58(10): 1–166. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H. 2009. Corpus linguistics in morphology: Morphological productivity. In Corpus Linguistics: An International Handbook, A. Lüdeling & M. Kytö (eds), 899–919. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Bauer, L. 1983. English Word-Formation. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2004. The function of word-formation and the inflection-derivation distinction. In Words in their Places. A Festschrift for J. Lachlan Mackenzie, H. Aersten, M. Hannay & R. Lyall (eds), 283–292. Amsterdam: Vrije Universiteit.Google Scholar
Becker, J. A. 1994. ‘Sneak-shoes’, ‘sworders’, and ‘nose-beards’: A case study of lexical innovation. First Language 14: 195–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Behrens, H. 1998. How difficult are complex verbs? Evidence from German, Dutch and English. Linguistics 36(4): 679–712. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2006. The input-output relations in first language acquisition. Language and Cognitive Processes 21: 2–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A. 1999. Children’s innovative verbs vs. nouns: Structured elicitations and spontaneous coinages. In Methods for Studying Language Production, L. Menn & N. Bernstein-Ratner (eds), 69–93. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
2004. Between emergence and mastery: The long developmental route of language acquisition. In Language Development across Childhood and Adolescence [Trends in Language Acquisition Research 3], R. A. Berman (ed.), 9–34. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bittner, D., Dressler, W. U. & Kilani-Schoch, M. (eds) 2003. Development of Verb Inflection in First Language Acquisition: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. L. 1985. Morphology: A Study of the Relation between Meaning and Form [Typological Studies in Language 9]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1995. Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes 10(5): 425–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2010. Language, Usage, and Cognition. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Bates, E., Casadio, P., Fenson, J., Fenson, L., Sanderl, L. & Weir, J. 1995. A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development. Cognitive Development 10: 159–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. 1993. The Lexicon in Acquisition. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2010. Adult offer, word-class, and child uptake in early lexical acquisition. First Language 30: 250–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2014. Acquisition of derivational morphology. In The Oxford Handbook of Derivational Morphology, R. Lieber & P. Štekauer (eds), 424–439. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
2016. The Lexicon in Acquisition, 3rd edn. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. & Hecht, B. F. 1982. Learning to coin agent and instrument nouns. Cognition 12: 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dhillon, R. 2010. Examining the ‘noun bias’: A structural approach. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 16: 51–60.Google Scholar
Doleschal, U. & Thornton, A. (eds) 2001. Extragrammatical and Marginal Morphology. Munich: Lincom.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. U. 2007. Productivity in word formation. In The Mental Lexicon: Core Perspectives, G. Jarema & G. Libben (eds), 159–183. Amsterdam: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2010. A typological approach to first language acquisition. In Language Acquisition Across Linguistic and Cognitive Systems [Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 52], M. Kail & M. Hickmann (eds), 109–124. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Dziubalska-Kołaczyk, K., Gagarina, N. & Kilani-Schoch, M. 2005. Reduplication in child language. In Studies on Reduplication, B. Hurch (ed.), 455–474. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
2014. Reduplication, repetition, hypercharacterisation and other affix-doubling in child language. In Affix Ordering across Languages and Frameworks, S. Manova (ed.), 259–276. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Ketrez, F. N. & Kilani-Schoch, M. (eds) 2017. Nominal Compound Acquisition [Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 61]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U. & Kilani-Schoch, M. 2017. Natural morphology. In The Cambridge Handbook of Morphology, A. Hippisley & G. Stump (eds), 356–389. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Kilani-Schoch, M. & Klampfer, S. 2003. How does a child detect morphology? Evidence from production. In Morphological Structure in Language Processing [Trends in Linguistics. Studies and Monographs 151], R. H. Baayen & R. Schreuder (eds), 391–425. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U. & Ladányi, M. 2000. Productivity in word formation (WF): A morphological approach. Acta Linguistica Hungarica 47: 103–144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Lettner, L. E. & Korecky-Kröll, K. 2010. First language acquisition of compounds. With special emphasis on early German child language. In Cross-Disciplinary Issues in Compounding [Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 311], S. Scalise & I. Vogel (eds), 323–344. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2012. Acquisition of German diminutive formation and compounding in a comparative perspective: Evidence for typology and the role of frequency. In Current Issues in Morphological Theory. (Ir)regularity, Analogy and Frequency [Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 322], F. Kiefer, M. Ladányi & P. Siptár (eds), 237–264. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Libben, G. & Korecky-Kröll, K. 2014. Conflicting vs. convergent vs. interdependent motivations in morphology. In Competing Motivations in Grammar & Usage, B. MacWhinney, A. Malchukov & E. Moravcsik (eds), 181–196. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Mayerthaler, W., Panagl, O. & Wurzel, W. U. 1987. Leitmotifs in Morphology [Studies in Language Companion Series 10]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Edelman, S. & Waterfall, H. 2007. Behavioral and computational aspects of language and its acquisition. Physics of Life Reviews 4: 253–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, L. R., Cassidy, K., Nappa, R., Papafragou, A. & Trueswell, J. C. 2005. Hard words. Language Learning and Development 1(1): 23–64. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Griffin, Z. M. 2002. Recency effects for meaning and form in word selection. Brain and Language 80: 465–487. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gülzow, I. & Gagarina, N. (eds) 2007. Frequency Effects in Language Acquisition. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hall, C. J. 2000. Prefixation, suffixation and circumfixation. In Morphologie / Morphology: Ein internationales Handbuch zur Flexion und Wortbildung / An international Handbook on Inflection and Word Formation, G. E. Booij, C. Lehmann, C. J. Hall & J. Mugdan (eds), 535–545. Berlin: De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, M. 1992. Language Experience and Early Language Development: From Input to Uptake. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, M. 1996. Word-class changing inflection in morphological theory. In Yearbook of Morphology 1995, G. E. Booij & J. van Marle (eds), 43–66. Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2006. Against markedness (and what to replace it with). Journal of Linguistics 42: 25–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hathout, N. & Namer, F. 2019. Paradigms in word formation: What are we up to? Morphology 29: 153–165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hempel, C. & Oppenheim, P. 1936. Der Typenbegriff im Lichte der neuen Logik (The Concept of Type in the Light of the New Logic). Leiden: Sijthoff.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. 1939/1968. Child Language, Aphasia, and Phonological Universals. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Kilani-Schoch, M., Balčiūnienė, I., Korecky-Kröll, K., Laaha, S. & Dressler, W. U. 2009. On the role of pragmatics in child-directed speech for the acquisition of verb morphology. Journal of Pragmatics 41: 219–239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Klamer, M. 2001. Expressives and iconicity in the lexicon. In Ideophones, E. F. K. Voeltz & C. Kilian-Hatz (eds), 165–181. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langer, J. 2001. The mosaic evolution of cognitive and linguistic ontogeny. In Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, M. Bowerman & S. C. Levinson (eds), 19–44. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laws, J. & Ryder, C. 2018. Register variation in spoken British English: The case of verb-forming suffixation. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 23(1): 1–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lo Duca, M. G. 1990. Creatività e regole. Studio sull’acquisizione della morfologia derivativa dell’Italiano (Creativity and rules. Study on the acquisition of derivational morphology of Italian). Bologna: Il Mulino.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. 2004. A multiple process solution to the logical problem of language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 31: 883–914. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mattiello, E. 2013. Extra-Grammatical Morphology in English. Abbreviations, Blends, Reduplicatives and Related Phenomena. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R., Myles, F. & Marsden, E. 2013. Second Language Learning Theories. New York NY: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norcliffe, E., Harris, A. & Jaeger, F. 2015. Linguistic psycholinguistics and its critical role in theory development: Early beginnings and recent advances. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 30(9): 1009–1032. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peters, A. M. 1997. Language typology, prosody, and the acquisition of grammatical morphemes. In The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition, D. I. Slobin (ed.), 135–197. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Posner, R. 1996. The Romance Languages. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Pounder, A. 2000. Processes and Paradigms in Word-Formation Morphology. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rainer, F. 2010. Carmens Erwerb der deutschen Wortbildung (Carmen’s acquisition of German word formation). Wien: Verlag der Österreichischen Akademie der Wissenschaften. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ravid, D. & Avidor, A. 1998. Acquisition of derived nominals in Hebrew: Developmental and linguistic principles. Journal of Child Language 25: 229–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ravid, D., Dressler, W. U., Nir-Sagiv, B., Korecky-Kröll, K., Souman, A., Rehfeldt, K., Laaha, S., Bertl, J., Basbøll, H. & Gillis, S. 2008. Core morphology in child directed speech: Crosslinguistic corpus analyses of noun plurals. In Corpora in Language Acquisition Research [Trends in Language Acquisition Research 6], H. Behrens (ed.), 25–60. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salerni, N., Assanelli, A., D’Odorico, L. & Rossi, G. 2007. Qualitative aspects of productive vocabulary at the 200- and 500-word stages: A comparison between spontaneous speech and parental report data. First Language 27: 75–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Savickienė, I. & Dressler, W. U. (eds) 2007. The Acquisition of Diminutives: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective [Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 43]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Say, S. 2005. Antipassive sja-verbs in Russian: Between inflection and derivation. In Morphology and its Demarcations [Current Issues in Linguistic Theory 264], W. U. Dressler, D. Kastovsky, O. Pfeiffer & F. Rainer (eds), 253–275. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soderstrom, M. 2007. Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants. Developmental Review 27(4): 501–532. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spencer, A. & Luís, A. R. 2012. Clitics. An Introduction. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stephany, U. 2012. Selecting a theoretical framework fitting acquisition data is no easy matter. In Selected Papers of the 10th International Conference of Greek Linguistics, Z. Gavriilidou, A. Efthymiou, E. Thomadaki & P. Kambakis-Vougiouklis (eds), 89–100. Komotini: Democritus University of Thrace.Google Scholar
2017. Contrastive lexical typology of German and Greek child speech and child-directed speech. In Nominal Compound Acquisition [Language Acquisition and Language Disorders 61], W. U. Dressler, F. N. Ketrez & M. Kilani-Schoch (eds), 275–286. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stephany, U. & Thomadaki, E. 2017. Compounding in early Greek language acquisition. In Dressler, Ketrez & Kilani-Schoch (eds), 119–143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stephany, U. & Voeikova, M. (eds) 2009. Development of Nominal Inflection in First Language Acquisition: A Cross-Linguistic Perspective. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Swan, D. 2000. How to build a lexicon: A case study of lexical errors and innovations. First Language 20: 187–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2003. Constructing a Language: A Usage-based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tribushinina, E., Voeikova, M. D. & Noccetti, S. (eds) 2015. Semantics and Morphology of Early Adjectives in First Language Acquisition. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Vapnarsky, V. & Veneziano, E. (eds) 2017. Lexical Polycategoriality. Cross-Linguistic, Cross-Theoretical and Language Acquisition Approaches [Studies in Language Companion Series 182]. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Xanthos, A., Laaha, S., Gillis, S., Stephany, U., Aksu-Koç, A., Christofidou, A., Gagarina, N., Hržica, G., Ketrez, F. N., Kilani-Schoch, M., Korecky-Kröll, K., Kovačević, M., Laalo, K., Palmović, M., Pfeiler, B., Voeikova, M. D. & Dressler, W. U. 2011. On the role of morphological richness in the early development of noun and verb inflection. First Language 31: 461–479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (1)

Cited by one other publication

Forshaw, William
2022. Book Review: Veronica Mattes, Sabine Sommer-Lolei, Katharina Korecky-Kröll and Wolfgang U. Dressler (Eds.), The acquisition of derivational morphology: A cross-linguistic perspective. First Language 42:5  pp. 696 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.