References
Arnett, C., & Jernigan, H.
(2014) A cognitive grammar account of case for L2 students of German. German as a Foreign Language, 11, 68–93.Google Scholar
Arnett, C., & Lysinger, D.
(2013) A cognitive grammar approach to teaching the Russian case system. Russian Language Journal/Русский язык, 631, 135–168.Google Scholar
Baten, K., & Lochtman, K.
(2014) Das deutsche Kasussystem im Fremdsprachenerwerb: Ein Forschungsüberblick. MUTTERSPRACHE, 124(1), 1–25.Google Scholar
Bley-Vroman, R.
(1989) What is the logical problem of foreign language learning? In S. Gass and J. Schachter (Eds.) Linguistic perspectives on second language acquisition (pp. 41–48). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H. & Muysken, P.
(1996) How adult second language learning differs from child first language development: Commentary on Epstein et al. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 191, 721–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davydov, V. V.
(1990) Types of generalization in instruction: Logical and psychological problems in the structuring of school curricula. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.Google Scholar
De Rycker, A., & De Knop, S.
(2017) Integrating Cognitive Linguistics and foreign language teaching: Historical background and new developments. Journal of Modern Languages, 19(1), 30–46.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C.
(2008) Usage-based and form-focused language acquisition: The associative learning of constructions, learned-attention, and the limited L2 endstate. In P. Robinson & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 372–405). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Epstein, S., Flynn, S., & Martohardjono, G.
(1996) Second language acquisition: Theoretical and experimental issues in contemporary research. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 191, 677–714. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, C. J.
(1967) The case for case. In E. Bach and R. T. Harms (Eds.), Universals in Linguistic Theory, (pp. 1–88). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Goad, H., & White, L.
(2004) Ultimate attainment of L2 inflection: Effects of L1 prosodic structure. EUROSLA Yearbook, 4(1), 119–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E.
(1995) Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Jackson, C. N.
(2007) Information, word order, and case markings during comprehension by L2 Learners of German. The Modern Language Journal, 91(3), 418–432. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jordens, P.
(1992) The cognitive function of case marking in German as a native and a foreign language. In S. Gass & L. Selinker (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp. 138–175). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kempe, V. & MacWhinney, B.
(1998) The acquisition of case marking by adult learners of Russian and German. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 20(4), 543–587. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1999) Processing of morphological and semantic cues in Russian and German. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(2), 129–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Köpcke, K. & Zubin, D.
(1983) Die kognitive Organisation der Genuszuweisung zu den einsilbigen Nomen der deutschen Gegenwartssprache. Zeitschrift für germanistische Linguistik II1, 166–182.Google Scholar
(1984) Sechs Prinzipien für die Genuszuweisung im Deutschen: Ein Beitrag zur natürlichen Klassifikation. Linguistische Berichte, 931, 26–50.Google Scholar
Liamkina, O.
(2005) The role of explicit meaning-based instruction in foreign language pedagogy: Applications of Cognitive Linguistics to teaching the German dative case to advanced learners (Doctoral dissertation). Available from Dissertations and Theses database (UMI No. 3193301).Google Scholar
Liamkina, O. & Ryshina-Pankova, M.
(2012) Grammar dilemma: Teaching grammar as a resource for meaning making. The Modern Language Journal 96(2), 270–289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lipka, L.
(1976) Topicalization, case grammar and lexical decomposition in English. Archivum Linguisticum, 71, 118–141.Google Scholar
Long, M. H.
(1991) Focus on form: A design feature in language teaching methodology. In K. de Bot, R. B. Ginsberg, & C. Kramsch (Eds), Foreign language research in cross-cultural perspective (pp. 39–52). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lysinger, D.
(2015) The case for hidden meaning: An application of cognitive linguistics in the Russian classroom. In K. Masuda, C. Arnett, & A. Labarca (Eds.), Cognitive Linguistics and Sociocultural Theory: Applications for second and foreign language teaching (pp. 233–258). Berlin: de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(1978) The acquisition of morpho-phonology. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 43(1).Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. J., Leinbach, J., Taraban, R., & McDonald, J. L.
(1989) Language learning: Cues or rules? Journal of Memory and Language, 281, 255–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meisel, J. M., Clahsen, H., & Pienemann, M.
(1981) On determining developmental stages in natural second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 31, 109–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mills, A.
(1986) The acquisition of gender: A study of English and German. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Negueruela, E., & Lantolf, J.
(2006) Concept-based instruction and the acquisition of L2 Spanish. In R. Salaberry and B. A. Lafford (Eds.) The art of teaching Spanish: Second language acquisition from research to praxis (pp. 79–102). Buffalo, NY: University of Buffalo Center for Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
Pallotti, G.
(2007) An operational definition of the emergence criterion. Applied Linguistics, 28 (3), 361–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pienemann, M.
(1998) Language processing and second language development: Processability theory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plonsky, L. & Oswald, F.
(2014) How big is “big”? Interpreting effect sizes in L2 research. Language Learning, 64(4), 878–912. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rankin, T.
Ritterbusch, R., LaFond, L., & Agustin, M.
(2006) Learner difficulties with German case: Implications from an action research study. Die Unterrichtspraxis/Teaching German, 391, 30–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P.
(1995) Attention, memory, and the ‘noticing’ hypothesis. Language Learning, 451, 283–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Task complexity, theory of mind, and intentional reasoning: effects on L2 speech production, interaction, uptake and perceptions of task difficulty. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 45(3), 193–213. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salimi, A., & Dadashpour, S.
(2012) Task complexity and language production dilemmas (Robinson’s Cognition Hypothesis vs. Skehan’s Trade-off Model). Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 461, 643–652. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R.
(1990) The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 111, 129–158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994) Deconstructing consciousness in search of useful definitions for applied linguistics. AILA Review, 111, 11–26.Google Scholar
(1995) Consciousness and foreign language learning: A tutorial on the role of attention and awareness in learning. In R. Schmidt (Ed.) Attention and awareness in foreign language learning (pp. 1–63). Honolulu, HI: Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center, University of Hawai’i.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. D., & Sprouse, R.
(1994) Word order and nominative case in nonnative language acquisition: a longitudinal study of (L1 Turkish) German interlanguage. Language Acquisition Studies in Generative Grammar, 31(4), 71–89.Google Scholar
Thielmann, W.
(2007) Fallstudie: Kasus in Sprachtheorie und Sprachvermittlung. Zielsprache Deutsch, 34(3), 11–34.Google Scholar
Tracy, R.
(1986) The acquisition of case morphology in German. Linguistics, 24(1), 47–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Compernolle, R. A., Gomez-Laich, M. P., & Weber, A.
(2016) Teaching L2 Spanish sociopragmatics through concepts: A classroom-based study. The Modern Language Journal, 100(1), 341–361. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B. & Borst, S.
(2012) The roles of explicit information and grammatical sensitivity in Processing Instruction: Nominative-Accusative case marking and word order in German L2. Foreign Language Annals, 45(1), 92–109.Google Scholar
VanPatten, B., Collopy, E., Price, J. E., Borst, S., & Qualin, A.
(2013) Explicit information, grammatical sensitivity, and the first-noun principle: A cross-linguistic study in Processing Instruction. The Modern Language Journal, 97(2), 506–527. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Walter, D. R., & van Compernolle, R. A.
(2017) Teaching German declension as meaning: a concept-based approach. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching, 11(1), 68–85. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L.
(1986) Markedness and parameter setting: some implications for a theory of adult second language acquisition. In F. R. Eckman, E. A. Moravcsik, & J. R. Wirth (Eds) Markedness (pp. 309-327). Boston, MA: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zubin, D. & Köpcke, K.
(1981) Gender: a less than arbitrary grammatical category. Chicago Linguistic Society 171, 439–449.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Walter, Daniel R.
2023. Boundary Crossing from the Start: 55 Years of Second Language Grammatical Gender Research in Review. In Crossing Boundaries in Researching, Understanding, and Improving Language Education [Educational Linguistics, 58],  pp. 57 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 3 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.