Word order in the input to Argentinian Spanish-learning children
How reliable as a cue to clause transitivity?
Languages express events in the world by means of transitive and intransitive constructions, whose properties
differ according to language typology. Children witness how specific languages express transitivity by listening to linguistic
input, which varies according to contextual variables (such as the age of the speaker and the addressee). In this study, we
investigated word order, one feature that typically helps discriminate between transitive and intransitive constructions but is
more reliable in some languages than others. The frequency and consistency of word order as a cue towards clause transitivity was
analysed in the input to 19 monolingual Spanish-learning children from Argentina (20 months old on average,
SD = 0.3), diverse in terms of socioeconomic status (SES). We found that some word orders occur far more
frequently and/or indicate clause transitivity much more reliably than others. In addition, their consistency as transitivity cues
varied across the registers and was crucially affected by SES.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 1.1Acquisition of transitive vs. intransitive patterns in production and comprehension
- 1.2The acquisition of word order across languages
- 1.3The role of the input registers and SES level
- 2.The present study
- 3.Methods
- 3.1Data
- 3.2Procedures
- 3.2.1Sample selection
- 3.2.2Clause segmentation
- 3.2.3Coding
- 3.3Analysis
- 3.3.1Cue frequency
- 3.3.2Cue reliability
- 4.Results
- 4.1Cue frequency
- 4.1.1Socioeconomic status (SES)
- 4.1.2Speaker
- 4.1.3Addressee
- 4.2Cue consistency
- 4.2.1Socioeconomic status (SES)
- 4.2.2Speaker
- 4.2.3Addressee
- 5.Discussion
- 5.1Variability in reliability and consistency of word order cues in the input
- 5.2The role of SES in the reliability and frequency of word order cues
- 5.3The role of the speaker and the addressee in the reliability and frequency of word order cues
- 5.4The combined effect of SES and input register on the frequency and reliability of word order cues: Some settings are more
favourable than others
- Notes
-
References
References (79)
References
Abbot-Smith, K., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2001). What
preschool children do and do not do with ungrammatical word orders. Cognitive
Development,
16
(2), 679–692.
Aguado-Orea, J. J., Witherstone, H., Bourgeois, L., & Baselga, A. (2019). Learning
to construct sentences in Spanish: A replication of the Weird Word Order technique. Journal of
Child
Language,
46
(6), 1249–1259.
Akhtar, N. (1999). Acquiring
basic word order: Evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structure. Journal of Child
Language,
26
(2), 339–356.
Akhtar, N. (2005). The
robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental
Science,
8
(2), 199–209.
Alam, F., Rosemberg, C., Garber, L., & Stein, A. (2021). Variation
sets in the speech directed to toddlers in Argentinian households. SES and type of activity
effects. Journal of Child
Language, 1–25.
Audisio, C. P., & Migdalek, M. J. (2020). Do
simple syntactic heuristics to verb meaning hold up? Testing the structure mapping account over spontaneous speech to
Spanish-learning children. Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de
linguistique,
65
(4), 556–582.
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting
linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical
Software,
67
(1), 1–48.
Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1982). Functionalist
approaches to grammar. In E. Wanner & L. Gleitman (Eds.), Language
acquisition: The state of the
art, (pp. 173–218). Cambridge University Press.
Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1989). Functionalism
and the competition model. In B. MacWhinney, & E. Bates (Eds.). The
crosslinguistic study of sentence
processing (pp. 3–73). Cambridge University Press.
Bates, E., MacWhinney, B., Caselli, C., Devescovi, A., Natale, F., & Venza, V. (1984). A
cross-linguistic study of the development of sentence interpretation strategies. Child
Development,
55
1, 341–54.
Bavin, E., & Growcott, C. (2000). Infants
of 24– 30 months understand verb frames. In M. Perkins & S. Howard (Eds.), New
directions in language development and
disorders (pp. 169–177). Kluwer Academic Publishing/Plenum.
Berman, R. A. (1993). Marking
of verb transitivity by Hebrew-speaking children. Journal of Child
Language,
20
1, 641–641.
Bernard, C., & Gervain, J. (2012). Prosodic
cues to word order: What level of representation? Frontiers in
Psychology, 31.
Brooks, P. J., & Tomasello, M. (1999). How
children constrain their argument structure
constructions. Language,
75
(4), 720–738.
Casillas, M., Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (2020). Early
language experience in a Tzeltal Mayan village. Child
Development,
91
(5), 1819–1835.
Casillas, M., Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (2021). Early
language experience in a Papuan community. Journal of Child
Language,
48
(4), 792–814.
Charvillat, A., & Kail, M. (1991). The
status of ‘canonical SVO sentences’ in French: A developmental study of the on-line processing of dislocated
sentences. Journal of Child
Language,
18
(3), 591–608.
Cherici, A., Chang, Y., & Tanaka, N. (2019). Evaluation
of cues in L1 Chinese input and output: A competition model approach to corpus data [Poster
presentation]. NINJAL-UHM Linguistics Workshop on Syntax-Semantics Interface, Language Acquisition
and Naturalistic Data Analysis, Mānoa, Honolulu, Hawai’i, United
States.
Devescovi, A., D’Amico, S., Smith, S., Mimica, I., & Bates, E. (1998). The
development of sentence comprehension in Italian and Serbo-Croatian: Local versus distributed
cues. In D. Hillert (Ed.), Sentence
processing: A cross-linguistic
perspective (pp. 345–377). Academic Press.
Dixon, R. M., & Aikhenvald, A. Y. (Eds.). (2000). Changing
valency: Case studies in transitivity. Cambridge University Press.
Du Bois, J. W. (1987). The
discourse basis of
ergativity. Language,
63
(4), 805–855.
Fisher, C., Gertner, Y., Scott, R. M., & Yuan, S. (2010). Syntactic
bootstrapping. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive
Science,
1
(2), 143–149.
Fleiss, J. L. (1971). Measuring
nominal scale agreement among many raters. Psychological
Bulletin,
76
(5), 378.
Franck, J., Millotte, S., & Lassotta, R. (2011). Early
word order representations: Novel arguments against old contradictions. Language
Acquisition,
18
(2), 121–135.
Friedman, J., Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (2010). Regularization
paths for generalized linear models via coordinate descent. Journal of Statistical
Software,
33
(1), 1–22. URL [URL].
Garcia, R., Roeser, J., & Höhle, B. (2019). Thematic
role assignment in the L1 acquisition of Tagalog: Use of word order and morphosyntactic
markers. Language
Acquisition,
26
(3), 235–261.
Garcia, R., Roeser, J., & Höhle, B. (2020). Children’s
online use of word order and morphosyntactic markers in Tagalog thematic role assignment: An eye-tracking
study. Journal of Child
Language,
47
(3), 533–555.
Gertner, Y., Fisher, C., & Eisengart, J. (2006). Learning
words and rules: Abstract knowledge of word order in early sentence
comprehension. Psychological
Science,
17
(8), 684–691.
Gervain, J., Nespor, M., Mazuka, R., Horie, R., & Mehler, J. (2008). Bootstrapping
word order in prelexical infants: A Japanese–Italian cross-linguistic study. Cognitive
Psychology,
57
(1), 56–74.
Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions:
A construction grammar approach to argument structure. University of Chicago Press.
Goldberg, A. (2006). Constructions
at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford University Press.
Goldfield, B. A. (2000). Nouns
before verbs in comprehension vs. production: The view from pragmatics. Journal of Child
Language,
27
(3), 501–520.
Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Cauley, K. M., & Gordon, L. (1987). The
eyes have it: Lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child
Language,
14
(1), 23–45.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful
differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H Brookes Publishing.
Haspelmath, M. (2015). Transitivity
prominence. In A. Malchukov & B. Comrie (Eds.), Introducing
the framework, and case studies from Africa and
Eurasia (Vol. 11) (pp. 131–148). Mouton De Gruyter.
Hernández, A. E., Sierra, I., & Bates, E. (2000). Sentence
interpretation in bilingual and monolingual Spanish speakers: Grammatical processing in a monolingual
mode. Spanish Applied
Linguistics,
4
(2), 179–213.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. (1991). Language
comprehension: A new look at some old themes. In N. A. Krasnegor, D. M. Rumbaugh, R. L. Schiefelbusch, & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Biological
and behavioral determinants for language
development (pp. 301–320). Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (1996). The
intermodal preferential looking paradigm: A window onto emerging language
comprehension. In D. McDaniel, C. McKee, & H. S. Cairns (Eds.), Methods
for assessing children’s
syntax (pp. 105–124). MIT Press.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R., & Naigles, L. (1996). Young
children’s ability to use syntactic frames to derive
meaning. In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R. Golinkoff (Eds.), The
origins of grammar: Evidence from early language
comprehension (pp. 123–158). MIT Press.
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1986). Function
and structure in maternal speech: Their relation to the child’s development of
syntax. Developmental
Psychology,
22
(2), 155.
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother-child
conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child
Development,
62
(4), 782–796.
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). The
relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language
development. Applied
Psycholinguistics,
19
(4), 603–629.
Hopper, P. J., & Thompson, S. A. (1980). Transitivity
in grammar and
discourse. Language
56
(2), 251–299.
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources
of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive
Psychology,
61
(4), 343–365.
Jackson-Maldonado, D., Peña, E., & Aghara, R. (2011). Funciones
de lenguaje y tipos de palabras en la interacción entre madres y sus hijos e
hijas. In C. Rojas Nieto & D. Jackson-Maldonado (Eds.), Interacción
y uso lingüístico en el desarrollo de la lengua
materna (pp. 27–61). UNAM-UAQ.
Kahane, H., & Kahane, R. (1950). The
position of the actor expression in colloquial Mexican
Spanish. Language,
26
(2), 236–263.
Kail, M. (1989). Cue
validity, cue cost and processing types in sentence comprehension in French and
Spanish. In B. MacWhinney & E. Bates (Eds.), The
crosslinguistic study of sentence
processing (pp. 77–117). Cambridge University Press.
Kail, M. (2004). On-line
grammaticality judgments in French children and adults: A crosslinguistic perspective. Journal
of Child
Language,
31
(3), 713–737.
Kail, M., Boibieux, M., & Coulaud, H. (2005). Early
comprehension of transitive and intransitive French
sentences. In B. Bokus (Ed.). Studies
in the psychology of child
language (pp. 195–226). Matrix.
Kail, M., & Charvillat, A. (1988). Local
and topological processing in sentence comprehension by French and Spanish children. Journal of
Child
Language,
15
1, 637–662.
Li, P., Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1993). Processing
a language without inflections: A reaction time study of sentence interpretation in
Chinese. Journal of Memory and
Language,
32
(2), 169–192.
Lieven, E. (2010). Input
and first language acquisition: Evaluating the role of
frequency. Lingua,
120
(11), 2546–2556.
MacWhinney, B. (1987). The
competition model. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms
of language
acquisition (pp. 249–308). Lawrence Erlbaum.
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The
CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. 3rd Edition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
MacWhinney, B. (2005). Extending
the competition model. International Journal of
Bilingualism,
9
(1), 69–84.
MacWhinney, B., Bates, E., & Kliegl, R. (1984). Cue
validity and sentence interpretation in English, German, and Italian. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal
Behavior,
23
(2), 127–150.
MacWhinney, B., & Bates, E. (Eds) (1989). The
crosslinguistic study of sentence processing. Cambridge University Press.
MacWhinney, B., Pleh, C., & Bates, E. (1985). The
development of sentence interpretation in Hungarian. Cognitive
Psychology,
17
(2), 178–209.
Mastin, J. D., Marchman, V., Elwood-Lowe, M., & Fernald, A. (2016). Quantity
& quality of child-directed speech (CDS) predict children’s vocabulary size and language processing
abilities. 20th Biennial Meeting of the International Conference on Infant
Studies. Louisiana, USA.
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M. (2005). The
role of frequency in the acquisition of English word order. Cognitive
Development,
20
(1), 121–136.
Matthews, D., Lieven, E., Theakston, A., & Tomasello, M. (2007). French
children’s use and correction of weird word orders: A constructivist account. Journal of Child
Language,
34
(2), 381.
Morgan, L., & Goldstein, H. (2004). Teaching
mothers of low socioeconomic status to use decontextualized language during storybook
reading. Journal of Early
Intervention,
26
(4), 235–252.
Oshima-Takane, Y., Goodz, E., & Derevensky, J. L. (1996). Birth
order effects on early language development: Do second born children learn from overheard
speech? Child
Development,
67
(2), 621–634.
Parisse, C., & Le Normand, M. T. (2000). How
children build their morphosyntax: The case of French. Journal of Child
Language,
27
(2), 267–292.
Phillips, J. R. (1973). Syntax
and vocabulary of mothers’ speech to young children: Age and sex comparisons. Child
Development,
44
1, 182–185.
Pye, C. (1983). Mayan
telegraphese: Intonational determinants of inflectional development in Quiche
Mayan. Language,
59
(3), 583–604.
R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and
environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL [URL]
Ramírez, M. L., Ibañez, M. I., Migdalek, M., Stein, A., Mealla, M., & Rosemberg, C. (2019). La
función pragmática de las emisiones dirigidas al niño en el entorno del hogar: el impacto de la educación
materna. Lingüística,
35
(2), 271–288.
Reyes, I. (2003). A
study of sentence interpretation in Spanish monolingual children. First
Language,
23
(3), 285–309.
Rosemberg, C. R., Alam, F., Audisio, C. P., Ramirez, M. L., Garber, L., & Migdalek, M. J. (2020). Nouns
and verbs in the linguistic environment of Argentinian toddlers: Socioeconomic and context-related
differences. First
Language,
22
(3), 192–217.
Rosemberg, C. R., Alam, F., Stein, A., Migdalek, M., Menti, A., & Ojea, G. (2015–2016). El entorno lingüístico de niños pequeños argentinos [Language
environments of young Argentinian
children]. CONICET. [dataset]
Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed
speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary
skill. Journal of Child
Language,
35
(1), 185–205.
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A
longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary
development. Child
Development,
83
(5), 1762–1774.
Snow, C. E., Arlman-Rupp, A., Hassing, Y., Jobse, J., Joosten, J., & Vorster, J. (1976). Mothers’
speech in three social classes. Journal of Psycholinguistic
Research,
5
(1), 1–20.
Soderstrom, M. (2007). Beyond
babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal
infants. Developmental
Review,
27
(4), 501–532.
Soderstrom, M., Blossom, M., Foygel, R., & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Acoustical
cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants. Journal of Child
Language,
35
(4), 869–902.
Sperry, D. E., Sperry, L. L., & Miller, P. J. (2019). Reexamining
the verbal environments of children from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Child
Development,
90
(4), 1303–1318.
Stein, A., Menti, A. B., & Rosemberg, C. R. (2021). Socioeconomic
status differences in the linguistic environment: A study with Spanish-speaking populations in
Argentina. Early
Years, 1–15.
Cited by (1)
Cited by one other publication
Quiroga, Macarena Sol, Celia Renata Rosemberg & Florencia Alam
2023.
Impacto de las actividades cotidianas, las circunstancias socioeconómicas y el input lingüístico en el vocabulario de niños argentinos de 4 años.
Lingüística y Literatura 44:84
► pp. 205 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.