Article published In:
Language, Interaction and Acquisition
Vol. 14:2 (2023) ► pp.167190
References (49)
References
Axelsson, E. L., Churchley, K., & Horst, J. S. (2012). The right thing at the right time: why ostensive naming facilitates word learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 (88), 1–8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62 (5), 875–890. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1993). Early referential understanding: Infants’ ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology, 29 (5), 832–843. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D. A., Markman, E. M., Bill, B., Desjardins, R. N., Irwin, J. M., & Tidball, G. (1996). Infants’ reliance on a social criterion for establishing word–object relations. Child Development, 67 (6), 3135–3153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6–9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109 (9), 3253–3258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015). Early word comprehension in infants: Replication and extension. Language Learning and Development, 11 (4), 369–380. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bion, R. A., Borovsky, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word–object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30 months. Cognition, 126 (1), 39–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brandone, A. C., Pence, K. L., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2007). Action speaks louder than words: Young children differentially weight perceptual, social, and linguistic cues to learn verbs. Child Development, 78 (4), 1322–1342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. Norton.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G., & Cochran, E. (1980). Towards a mechanism of joint visual attention in human infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, 253–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 63 (4), 1–143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
D’Entremont, B., Hains, S. M., & Muir, D. W. (1997). A demonstration of gaze following in 3-to 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 20 (4), 569–572. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 991, 9602–9605. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., & Tylén, K. (2014). Dialog as interpersonal synergy. New Ideas in Psychology, 32 1, 147–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gliga, T., & Csibra, G. (2009). One-year-old infants appreciate the referential nature of deictic gestures and words. Psychological Science, 20 (3), 347–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. A. (2006). Baby wordsmith from associationist to social sophisticate. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15 (1), 30–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M., Mervis, C. B., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1994). Early object labels: The case for a developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language, 21 (1), 125–155. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hollich, G. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., Brand, R. J., Brown, E., Chung, H. L., … Bloom, L. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65 (3), i–vi, 1–123.Google Scholar
Horst, J. S., & Samuelson, L. K. (2008). Fast mapping but poor retention by 24-month-old infants. Infancy, 131, 128–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Houston-Price, C., Plunkett, K., & Duffy, H. (2006). The use of social and salience cues in early word learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95 1, 27–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krauzlis, R. J., Bollimunta, A., Arcizet, F., & Wang, L. (2014). Attention as an effect not a cause. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18 (9), 457–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization and naming in children. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Moore, C., Angelopoulos, M., & Bennett, P. (1999). Word learning in the context of referential and saliency cues. Developmental Psychology, 35 (1), 60–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muir, D., Hains, S., Cao, Y., & D’Entremont, B. (1996). 3- to 6-month-olds’ sensitivity to adult intentionality: The role of adult contingency and eye direction in dyadic interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 19 1, 199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nomikou, I., Rohlfing, K. J., Cimiano, P., & Mandler, J. M. (2018). Evidence for early comprehension of action verbs. Language Learning and Development, 15 (1), 64–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plunkett, K. (1997). Theories of early language acquisition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1 (4), 146–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pruden, S. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hennon, E. A. (2006). The birth of words: Ten-month-olds learn words through perceptual saliency. Child Development, 77 (2), 266–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Quine, W. V. O. (1960). Word and object. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rohlfing, K. J., Wrede, B., Vollmer, A.-L., & Oudeyer, P. Y. (2016). An alternative to mapping a word onto a concept in language acquisition: Pragmatic frames. Frontiers in Psychology, 7 (470), 1–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Samuelson, L. K., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Memory and attention make smart word learning: An alternative account of Akhtar, Carpenter, and Tomasello. Child Development, 69 (1), 94–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2008). Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology, 18 (9), 668–671. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sloetjes, H. & Wittenburg, P. (2008). Annotation by category: ELAN and ISO DCR. In Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (pp. 816–820). European Language Resources Association (ELRA).Google Scholar
Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S. & Landau, B. (1996). Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism? Cognition, 60 (2), 143–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. B., Colunga, E., & Yoshida, H. (2010). Knowledge as process: Contextually cued attention and early word learning. Cognitive Science, 34 (7), 1287–1314. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Suarez-Rivera, C., Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2019). Multimodal parent behaviors within joint attention support sustained attention in infants. Developmental Psychology, 55 (1), 96–109. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tincoff, R., & Jusczyk, P. W. (1999). Some beginnings of word comprehension in 6-month-olds. Psychological Science, 10 (2), 172–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). Six-month-olds comprehend words that refer to parts of the body. Infancy, 17 (4), 432–444. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). The social-pragmatic theory of word learning. Pragmatics, 10 (4), 401–413. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Akhtar, N. (1995). Two-year-olds use pragmatic cues to differentiate reference to objects and actions. Cognitive Development, 101, 201–224. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57 (6), 1454. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Treue, S. (2003). Visual attention: the where, what, how and why of saliency. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13 (4), 428–432. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Lin, Y., Armstrong III, B., Cartmill, E. A., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Gleitman, L. R. (2016). Perceiving referential intent: Dynamics of reference in natural parent–child interactions. Cognition, 148 1, 117–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wass, S. V., Clackson, K., Georgieva, S. D., Brightman, L., Nutbrown, R., & Leong, V. (2018). Infants’ visual sustained attention is higher during joint play than solo play: Is this due to increased endogenous attention control or exogenous stimulus capture? Developmental Science, 21 (6), e12667. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wass, S. V., Noreika, V., Georgieva, S., Clackson, K., Brightman, L., Nutbrown, R., … Leong, V. (2018). Parental neural responsivity to infants’ visual attention: How mature brains influence immature brains during social interaction. PLoS Biology, 16 (12), e2006328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F., Cohen, L. B., Lloyd, V. L., Casasola, M., & Stager, C. L. (1998). Acquisition of word–object associations by 14-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 34 (6), 1289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wildt, E., Rohlfing, K. J., & Scharlau, I. (2019). The role of saliency in learning first words. Frontiers in Psychology, 10 (1150), 1–8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Woodward, A. L., Markman, E. M., & Fitzsimmons, C. M. (1994). Rapid word learning in 13- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 30 (4), 553–566. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2016). The social origins of sustained attention in one-year-old human infants. Current Biology, 26 (9), 1235–1240. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yurovsky, D., & Frank, M. C. (2017). Beyond naïve cue combination: Saliency and social cues in early word learning. Developmental Science, 20 (2), e12349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar