References (53)
References
Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Augst, G. (1984). Kinderwort: Der aktive Kinderwortschatz (kurz vor der Einschulung). Frankfurt/Main: Lang.Google Scholar
Barz, I. (2009). Die Wortbildung. In Duden (Ed.), Die Grammatik. Vol. 41. (pp. 634–762). Mannheim: Dudenverlag.Google Scholar
Behrens, H. (1998). How difficult are complex verbs? Evidence from German, Dutch and English. Linguistics, 36 (4), 679–712. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003). Verbal prefixation in German child and adult language. Acta Linguistica Hungarica, 501, 41–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. (2004). Between emergence and mastery: The long developmental route of language acquisition. In R. Berman (Ed.), Language Development across childhood and adolescence (pp.9–34). Amsterdam: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Booij, G. (2010). Construction morphology. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1982). Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic development. In E. Wanner and L. R. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 319–346). Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Brown, R., and Berko, J. (1960). Word association and the acquisition of grammar. Child Development, 311, 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 101, 425–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Sonnenstuhl, I., and Blevins, J. P. (2003). Derivational morphology in the German mental lexicon: A dual mechanism account. In H. R. Baayen and R. Schreuder (Eds.), Morphological structure in language processing (pp. 125–555). Berlin: de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E., and Berman, R. (1984). Structure and use in the acquisition of word formation. Language, 60 (3), 542–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. (2009). First language acquisition. 2nd ed. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Comrie, B. (1985). Causative verb formation and other verb-deriving morphology. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Grammatical categories and the lexicon (pp. 309–348). Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Cordes, A. (2014). The role of frequency in children’s learning of morphological constructions. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Dewell, R. B. (2015). The semantics of German verb prefixes. Amsterdam: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dockrell, J. E. and Messer, D. (2004). Lexical Acquisition in the Early School Years. In R. Berman (Ed.), Language Development across childhood and adolescence (pp. 35–52). Amsterdam: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duden (1998). Grammatik der deutschen Gegenwartssprache. 6th ed. Mannheim: Dudenverlag.Google Scholar
Eichinger, L. M. (2003). Dependenz in der Wortbildung. In V. Ágel, L. M. Eichinger, H.-W. Eroms, P. Hellwig, H.-J. Heringer, and H. Lobin (Eds.), Dependenz und Valenz (Handbücher zur Sprach‑ und Kommunikationswissenschaft), (pp. 1065–1080), Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, P. (2013). Das Wort. Grundriss der deutschen Grammatik. 4th ed. Stuttgart: Metzler.Google Scholar
Elman, J. L. (1990). Finding structure in time. Cognitive Science, 14 (2), 179–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1993). Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small. Cognition, 481, 71–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elsen, H. (1999). Ansätze zu einer funktionalistisch-kognitiven Grammatik, Konsequenzen aus Regularitäten des Erstsprachenerwerbs. Tübingen: Niemeyer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fleischer, W. and Barz, I. (2012). Wortbildung der deutschen Gegenwartssprache. 4th ed. Berlin: de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gordon, P. (1989). Levels of affixation in the acquisition of English morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 281, 519–530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, M., and Sims, A. (2010). Understanding Morphology. 2nd. ed. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Havron, N., Ramus, F., Heude, B., Forhan, A., Cristia, A., and Peyre, H. (2019). The effect of older siblings on language development as a function of age difference and sex. To appear in Psychological Science. Preprint [URL]. DOI logo
Heide, J., Lorenz, A., Meinunger, A., and Burchert, F. (2010). The influence of morphological structure on the processing of German prefixed verbs. In A. Onysko and S. Michel (Eds.), Cognitive perspectives on word formation (pp. 375–396). Berlin: de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26 (1), 55–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986). Some fundamental aspects of language development after age 5. In P. Fletcher and M. Garman (Eds.), Language acquisition (pp. 455–474). Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kauschke, C., Nutsch, K., and Schrauf, J. (2012). Verarbeitung von konkreten und abstrakten Wörtern bei Kindern im Schulalter. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 44 (1), 2–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Korecky-Kröll, K. (2011). Der Erwerb der Nominalmorphologie bei zwei Wiener Kindern: Eine Untersuchung im Rahmen der Natürlichkeitstheorie. Univ. Wien: Diss.Google Scholar
Levie, R., Ben-Zvi, G., and Ravid, D. (2017). Morpho-lexical development in language-impaired and typically developing Hebrew-speaking children from two SES backgrounds. Reading and Writing, 301, 1035–1064. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mattes, V. (2018). Derivationsmorphologie und Wortarten im Erstspracherwerb des Deutschen. Mit einem Fokus auf dem Schuleintrittsalter. Univ. Graz: Habilitation thesis.Google Scholar
Mesch, B. (2015). Anknüpfungspunkte für eine Interface-Didaktik des Verbs – am Beispiel von Präfix‑ und Partikelverben. In B. Mesch and B. Rothstein (Eds.), Was tun mit dem Verb? (pp. 283–321). Berlin: de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W., Anderson, R. C., Schommer, M., Scott, J., and Stallman, A. (1989). Morphological families in the internal lexicon. Univ. of Illinois: Technical Reports. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nagy, W., Diakidoy, I., and Anderson, R. (1993). The acquisition of morphology: Learning the contribution of suffixes to the meanings of derivatives. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25 (2), 155–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nippold, M. (2007). Later Language development. School-age children, adolescents, and young adults. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Prime, H., Pauker, S., Plamandon, A., Perlman, M., and Jenkins, J. (2014). Sibship size, sibling sognitive sensitivity, and children’s receptive vocabulary. Pediatrics, 133 (2). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rainer, F. (2010). Carmens Erwerb der deutschen Wortbildung. Wien: Verlag der Österreichischen Akademie der Wissenschaften.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. (2004). Derivational morphology revisited: Later language development in Hebrew. In R. Berman (Ed.), Language Development across childhood and adolescence (pp. 53–81). Amsterdam: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Savage, C., Lieven, E., Theakstone, A., and Tomasello, M. (2003). Testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic representations: lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. Developmental Science, 6 (5), 557–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schipke, C., and Kauschke, C. (2011). Early word formation in German language acquisition: A study on word formation growth during the second and third year. First Language, 311, 67–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schulz, P., Wymann, K., and Penner, Z. (2001). The Early Acquisition of Verb Meaning in German by Normally Developing and Language Impaired Children. Brain and Language, 771, 407–418. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Seiler, Th. B., and Wannenmacher, W. (1983). How can we assess meaning and investigate meaning development: Theoretical and methodological considerations from an epistemological point of view. In Th. B. Seiler and W. Wannenmacher (Eds.), Concept Development and the Development of Word Meaning (pp. 320–339). Berlin: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smolka, E., Preller, K. H., and Eulitz, C. (2014). “Verstehen” (“understand”) primes “stehen” (“stand”): morphological structure overrides semantic compositionality in the lexical representation of German complex verbs. Journal of Memory and Language, 721, 16–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smolka, E., and Eulitz, C. (2018). Psycholinguistic measures for German verb pairs: Semantic transparency, semantic relatedness, verb family size, and age of reading acquisition. Behavior Reserach Methods, published online june 2018. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1995). Issues in the study of input: Fine-tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In P. Fletcher and B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Handbook of child language (pp. 108–193). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Szagun, G. (1983). Bedeutungsentwicklung beim Kind: Wie Kinder Wörter entdecken. München: Urban.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs. A case study of early grammatical development. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003). Constructing a Language. A Usage-based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tschirner, Erwin. (2010). Wortschatz. In H.-J. Krumm, C. Faydrich, B. Hufeisen, C. Riemer (Eds.), Deutsch als Fremd‑ und Zweitsprache 1. Ein Internationales Handbuch (pp. 236–245). Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Zifonun, G. (1973). Zur Theorie der Wortbildung am Beispiel deutscher Präfixverben. München: Hueber.Google Scholar
Cited by (3)

Cited by three other publications

Korecky-Kröll, Katharina, Sabine Sommer-Lolei, Veronika Mattes & Wolfgang U. Dressler
2022. Morphological Verb Families in German-Speaking Children and Parents of Two SES Backgrounds According to a Groundbreaking Model by Dorit Ravid and Colleagues. In Developing Language and Literacy [Literacy Studies, 23],  pp. 495 ff. DOI logo
Dressler, Wolfgang U., Veronika Mattes & Laila Kjærbæk
2021. Chapter 1. Introduction. In The Acquisition of Derivational Morphology [Language Acquisition and Language Disorders, 66],  pp. 2 ff. DOI logo
Sommer-Lolei, Sabine, Veronika Mattes, Katharina Korecky-Kröll & Wolfgang U. Dressler
2021. Chapter 5. Early phases of development of German derivational morphology. In The Acquisition of Derivational Morphology [Language Acquisition and Language Disorders, 66],  pp. 110 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.