A Generalization across the Verbal and Nominal Domains
Maya Yablonski | The Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University
Michal Ben-Shachar | Department of English Literature and Linguistics, Bar Ilan University
An extensive body of psycholinguistic research suggests that word reading involves morphological decomposition: Individual morphemes are extracted and lexically accessed when skilled readers are presented with multi-morphemic orthographic stimuli. This view is supported by the Morpheme Interference Effect (MIE): Responses to pseudowords that contain real morphemes are slower and less accurate than responses to pseudowords that contain invented morphemes. The MIE was previously demonstrated in several languages with linear morphologies. Here, we examined whether the MIE applies to Hebrew, a language with an interleaved morphology, and whether it generalizes across the nominal and verbal domains. Participants performed a lexical decision task on visually presented Hebrew words and pseudowords derived from real or invented roots. The results showed robust MIEs in both the verbal and nominal domains. Specifically, pseudowords derived from real roots induced significantly lower accuracy and longer response times compared to pseudowords derived from invented roots. Participants’ verbal and nominal MIEs were significantly correlated, suggesting that the MIE captures a general sensitivity to morphological structure.
Amenta, S., & Crepaldi, D. (2012). Morphological processing as we know it: An analytical review of morphological effects in visual word identification. Frontiers in Psychology, 31, 1–12.
Andrews, S. (1997). The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 41, 439–461.
Baayen, R.H., Milin, P., Đurđević, D.F., Hendrix, P., & Marelli, M. (2011). An amorphous model for morphological processing in visual comprehension based on naive discriminative learning. Psychological Review, 1181, 438–481.
Balota, D.A., Cortese, M.J., Sergent-Marshall, S.D., Spieler, D.H., & Yap, M.J. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1331, 283–316.
Bat-El, O. (1994). Stem modification and cluster transfer in Modern Hebrew. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 121, 571–596.
Bat-El, O. (2003). The fate of the consonantal root and the binyan in Optimality Theory. Recherches Linguistiques de Vincennes, 321, 31–60.
Beyersmann, E., Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2012). Morphological processing during visual word recognition in developing readers: Evidence from masked priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 651, 1306–1326.
Butterworth, B. (1983). Lexical representations. In B. Butterwoth (Ed.), Language production (pp. 257–294). London: Academic Press.
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 101, 425–455.
Caramazza, A., Laudanna, A., & Romani, C. (1988). Lexical access and inflectional morphology. Cognition, 281, 297–332.
Carlisle, J.F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 121, 169–190.
Casalis, S., Colé, P., & Sopo, D. (2004). Morphological awareness in developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 541, 114–138.
Casalis, S., Quémart, P., & Duncan, L.G. (2015). How language affects children’s use of derivational morphology in visual word and pseudoword processing: evidence from a cross-language study. Frontiers in Psychology, 61, 1–10.
Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, T., & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and performance VI (pp. 535–555). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Crepaldi, D., Rastle, K., & Davis, C.J. (2010). Morphemes in their place: Evidence for position-specific identification of suffixes. Memory & Cognition, 381, 312–321.
Crepaldi, D., Rastle, K., Davis, C.J., & Lupker, S.J. (2013). Seeing stems everywhere: position-independent identification of stem morphemes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 391, 510–525.
Deacon, S.H., & Kirby, J.R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 251, 223–238.
Deutsch, A. (2015). The role of the nominal-pattern morpheme in lexical access in Hebrew: Evidence from written-word perception and single-word production. In The 9th International Morphological Processing Conference (pp. 53–54). Potsdam, Germany.
Deutsch, A. (2016). The separability of morphological processes from semantic meaning and syntactic class in production of single words: Evidence from the Hebrew root morpheme. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 451, 1–28.
Deutsch, A., Frost, R., & Forster, K.I. (1998). Verbs and nouns are organized and accessed differently in the mental lexicon: Evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 241, 1238–1255.
Deutsch, A., Frost, R., Pelleg, S., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (2003). Early morphological effects in reading: evidence from parafoveal preview benefit in Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 101, 415–422.
Deutsch, A., Frost, R., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (2005). Morphological parafoveal preview benefit effects in reading: Evidence from Hebrew. Language and Cognitive Processes, 201, 341–371.
Deutsch, A., & Malinovitch, T. (2016). The role of the morpho-phonological word-pattern unit in single-word production in Hebrew. Journal of Memory and Language, 871, 1–15.
Deutsch, A., & Meir, A. (2011). The role of the root morpheme in mediating word production in Hebrew. Language and Cognitive Processes, 261, 716–744.
Efron, B., & Tibshirani, R.J. (1994). An introduction to the bootstrap. CRC press.
Elbro, C., & Arnbak, E. (1996). The role of morpheme recognition and morphological awareness in dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 461, 209–240.
Frost, R. (2012). Towards a universal model of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 351, 263–279.
Frost, R., Deutsch, A., & Forster, K.I. (2000). Decomposing morphologically complex words in a nonlinear morphology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 261, 751–765.
Frost, R., Deutsch, A., Gilboa, O., Tannenbaum, M., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2000). Morphological priming: Dissociation of phonological, semantic, and morphological factors. Memory & Cognition, 28(8), 1277–1288.
Frost, R., Forster, K.I., & Deutsch, A. (1997). What can we learn from the morphology of Hebrew? A masked-priming investigation of morphological representation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 231, 829–856.
Frost, R., Kugler, T., Deutsch, A., & Forster, K.I. (2005). Orthographic structure versus morphological structure: principles of lexical organization in a given language. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 311, 1293–326.
Frost, R., & Plaut, D.C. (2005). The word-frequency database for printed Hebrew. Available from [URL]
Fruchter, J., & Marantz, A. (2015). Decomposition, lookup, and recombination: MEG evidence for the Full Decomposition model of complex visual word recognition. Brain and Language, 1431, 81–96.
Giraudo, H., & Grainger, J. (2000). Effects of prime word frequency and cumulative root frequency in masked morphological priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 151, 421–444.
Giraudo, H., & Grainger, J. (2001). Priming complex words: Evidence for supralexical representation of morphology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 81, 127–131.
Grainger, J., Dufau, S., Montant, M., Ziegler, J.C., & Fagot, J. (2012). Orthographic processing in Baboons (Papio papio). Science, 3361, 245–248.
Horvath, J., & Siloni, T. (2009). Hebrew idioms: The organization of the lexical component. Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics, 11, 283–310.
Itai, A., & Wintner, S. (2008). Language resources for Hebrew. Language Resources and Evaluation, 421, 75–98.
Janssen, N., Bi, Y., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A tale of two frequencies: Determining the speed of lexical access for Mandarin Chinese and English compounds. Language and Cognitive Processes, 231, 1191–1223.
Jarvella, R.J., & Wennstedt, O. (1993). Recognition of partial regularity in words and sentences. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 341, 76–85.
Kavé, G., & Levy, Y. (2004). Preserved morphological decomposition in persons with Alzheimer’s Disease. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 471, 835–847.
Kavé, G., & Levy, Y. (2005). The processing of morphology in old age. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 481, 1442–1451.
Kirby, J.R., Deacon, S.H., Bowers, P.N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Woolley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability. Reading and Writing, 251, 389–410.
Kolan, L., Leikin, M., & Zwitserlood, P. (2011). Morphological processing and lexical access in speech production in Hebrew: Evidence from picture–word interference. Journal of Memory and Language, 651, 286–298.
Laudanna, A., Cermele, A., & Caramazza, A. (1997). Morpho-lexical representations in naming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 121, 49–66.
Leikin, M., & Even Zur, H. (2006). Morphological processing in adult dyslexia. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 351, 471–490.
Longtin, C.-M., & Meunier, F. (2005). Morphological decomposition in early visual word processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 531, 26–41.
Mahfoudhi, A., Elbeheri, G., Al-Rashidi, M., & Everatt, J. (2010). The role of morphological awareness in reading comprehension among typical and learning disabled native Arabic speakers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 431, 500–514.
Manelis, L., & Tharp, D.A. (1977). The processing of affixed words. Memory & Cognition, 51, 690–695.
Marslen-Wilson, W., Tyler, L.K., Waksler, R., & Older, L. (1994). Morphology and meaning in the English mental lexicon. Psychological Review, 1011, 3–33.
Martin, J., Frauenfelder, U.H., & Colé, P. (2014). Morphological awareness in dyslexic university students. Applied Psycholinguistics, 351, 1213–1233.
McBride-Chang, C., Wagner, R.K., Muse, A., Chow, B.W.-Y., & Shu, H. (2005). The role of morphological awareness in children’s vocabulary acquisition in English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 261, 415–435.
Meunier, F., & Longtin, C.-M. (2007). Morphological decomposition and semantic integration in word processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 561, 457–471.
Nagy, W., Berninger, V.W., & Abbott, R.D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 981, 134–147.
Rastle, K., & Davis, M.H. (2008). Morphological decomposition based on the analysis of orthography. Language and Cognitive Processes, 231, 942–971.
Rastle, K., Davis, M.H., Marslen-Wilson, W.D., & Tyler, L.K. (2000). Morphological and semantic effects in visual word recognition: A time-course study. Language and Cognitive Processes, 151, 507–537.
Rastle, K., Davis, M.H., & New, B. (2004). The broth in my brother’s brothel: morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 111, 1090–1098.
Ravid, D., & Mashraki, Y.E. (2007). Prosodic reading, reading comprehension and morphological skills in Hebrew-speaking fourth graders. Journal of Research in Reading, 301, 140–156.
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English–Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing, 211, 481–504.
Schiff, R., & Raveh, M. (2007). Deficient morphological processing in adults with developmental dyslexia: another barrier to efficient word recognition?Dyslexia, 131, 110–129.
Schiff, R., & Ravid, D. (2007). Morphological analogies in Hebrew-speaking university students with dyslexia compared with typically developing gradeschoolers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 361, 237–253.
Schiff, R., & Ravid, D. (2013). Morphological processing in Hebrew-speaking students with reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 461, 220–229.
Schiff, R., Schwartz-Nahshon, S., & Nagar, R. (2011). Effect of phonological and morphological awareness on reading comprehension in Hebrew-speaking adolescents with reading disabilities. Annals of Dyslexia, 611, 44–63.
Seidenberg, M.S. (1987). Sublexical structures in visual word recognition: Access units or orthographic redundancy? In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance 12: The psychology of reading (pp. 245–263). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Share, D.L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 1341, 584–615.
Siegel, L.S. (2008). Morphological awareness skills of English language learners and children with dyslexia. Topics in Language Disorders, 281, 15–27.
Sonnenstuhl, I., Eisenbeiss, S., & Clahsen, H. (1999). Morphological priming in the German mental lexicon. Cognition, 721, 203–236.
Taft, M., & Forster, K.I. (1975). Lexical storage and retrieval of prefixed words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 141, 638–647.
Traficante, D., Marcolini, S., Luci, A., Zoccolotti, P., & Burani, C. (2011). How do roots and suffixes influence reading of pseudowords: A study of young Italian readers with and without dyslexia. Language and Cognitive Processes, 261, 777–793.
Ussishkin, A. (1999). The inadequacy of the consonantal root: Modern Hebrew denominal verbs and output-output correspondence. Phonology, 161, 401–442.
Ussishkin, A. (2006). Affix-favored contrast inequity and psycholinguistic grounding for non-concatenative morphology. Morphology, 161, 107–125.
Velan, H., Deutsch, A., & Frost, R. (2013). The flexibility of letter-position flexibility: Evidence from eye movements in reading Hebrew. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 391, 1143–1152.
Velan, H., & Frost, R. (2007). Cambridge University versus Hebrew University: The impact of letter transposition on reading English and Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 141, 913–918.
Velan, H., & Frost, R. (2009). Letter-transposition effects are not universal: The impact of transposing letters in Hebrew. Journal of Memory and Language, 611, 285–302.
Velan, H., & Frost, R. (2011). Words with and without internal structure: What determines the nature of orthographic and morphological processing?Cognition, 1181, 141–156.
Velan, H., Frost, R., Deutsch, A., & Plaut, D.C. (2005). The processing of root morphemes in Hebrew: Contrasting localist and distributed accounts. Language and Cognitive Processes, 201, 169–206.
Yap, M.J., Sibley, D.E., Balota, D.A., Ratcliff, R., & Rueckl, J. (2015). Responding to nonwords in the lexical decision task: Insights from the English Lexicon Project. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 411, 597–613.
Yarkoni, T., Balota, D., & Yap, M. (2008). Moving beyond Coltheart’s N: A new measure of orthographic similarity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 151, 971–979.
Cited by (12)
Cited by 12 other publications
Solaja, Olga & Davide Crepaldi
2024. The role of morphology in novel word learning: a registered report. Royal Society Open Science 11:6
Laure, Yael & Sharon Armon-Lotem
2023. Hebrew-L2 speakers process auditory templatic words through their L1 processing mechanism with awareness of L2. Frontiers in Psychology 14
Laure, Yael & Sharon Armon-Lotem
2024. Linear and nonlinear processing of Hebrew templatic words: the role of metalinguistic awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics 45:4 ► pp. 666 ff.
De Rosa, Mara & Davide Crepaldi
2022. Letter chunk frequency does not explain morphological masked priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 29:2 ► pp. 589 ff.
Oganyan, Marina & Richard A. Wright
2022. The Role of the Root in Spoken Word Recognition in Hebrew: An Auditory Gating Paradigm. Brain Sciences 12:6 ► pp. 750 ff.
Kelelufna, Jusuf Haries
2021. Analisis Bahasa Kitab Kidung Agung: Suatu Upaya Melacak Peredaksian. DUNAMIS: Jurnal Teologi dan Pendidikan Kristiani 6:1 ► pp. 65 ff.
Beyersmann, Elisabeth, Petroula Mousikou, Ludivine Javourey-Drevet, Sascha Schroeder, Johannes C. Ziegler & Jonathan Grainger
2020. Morphological Processing across Modalities and Languages. Scientific Studies of Reading 24:6 ► pp. 500 ff.
Yablonski, Maya & Michal Ben-Shachar
2020. Sensitivity to word structure in adult Hebrew readers is associated with microstructure of the ventral reading pathways. Cortex 128 ► pp. 234 ff.
2018. The Form of Morphemes: MEG Evidence From Masked Priming of Two Hebrew Templates. Frontiers in Psychology 9
Yablonski, Maya, Uri Polat, Yoram S. Bonneh & Michal Ben-Shachar
2017. Microsaccades are sensitive to word structure: A novel approach to study language processing. Scientific Reports 7:1
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 december 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.