Article published In:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 12:1 (2017) ► pp.120
References

References

Bai, X., Yan, G., Liversedge, S. P., Zang, C., & Rayner, K.
(2008) Reading spaced and unspaced Chinese text: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 341, 1277–1287.Google Scholar
Bronk, M., Zwitserlood, P., & Bölte, J.
(2013) Manipulations of word frequency reveal differences in the processing of morphologically complex and simple words in German. Frontiers in Language Sciences, 41, 1–14.Google Scholar
Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M.
(2010) SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based on film subtitles. PloS ONE, 5(6), e10729. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, C., Huang, X., & Yu, H.
(2002) The processing mechanism of structurally symmetrical Chinese character recognition: An experimental research on radical-level process. Psychological Science (China), 251, 187–190.Google Scholar
Cui, L., Drieghe, D., Yan, G., Bai, X., Chi, H., & Liversedge, P. S.
(2013) Parafoveal processing across different lexical constituents in Chinese reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(2), 403–416. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cui, L., Yan, G., Bai, X., Hyönä, J., Wang, S., & Liversedge, P. S.
(2013) Processing of compound-word characters in reading Chinese: An eye-movement-contingent display change study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(3), 527–547. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drieghe, D., Pollatsek, A., Juhasz, B. J., & Rayner, K.
(2010) Parafoveal processing during reading is reduced across a morphological boundary. Cognition, 1161, 136–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feng, G.
(2006) Eye movements as time-series random variables: A stochastic model of eye movement control in reading. Cognitive Systems Research, 71, 70–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoosain, R.
(1992) Psychological reality of the word in Chinese. In H.-C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese (pp. 111–130). Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsu, S.-H., & Huang, K.-C.
(2000) Interword spacing in Chinese text layout. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 911, 355–365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huang, B., & Wei, L.
(2012) Modern Chinese (Chapter 4: Word), Peking University Press.Google Scholar
Inhoff, A. W., Briihl, D., & Schwartz, J.
(1996) Compound word effects differ in reading, on-line naming, and delayed naming tasks. Memory & Cognition, 241, 466–476. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, A. M., & Grainger, J.
(1992) Testing a semi-stochastic variant of the interactive activation model in different word recognition experiments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 181, 1174–1188.Google Scholar
Ji, H., Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L.
(2011) Benefits and costs of lexical decomposition and semantic integration during the processing of transparent and opaque English compounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 651, 406–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juhasz, B. J., Inhoff, A. W., & Rayner, K.
(2005) The role of interword spaces in the processing of English compound words. Language & Cognitive Processes, 201, 291–316. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lai, Y., & Myers, J.
(2012) The recognition of spoken mono-morphemic compounds in Chinese. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics, 101, 41–88.Google Scholar
Laine, M., Vainio, S., & Hyönä, J. (1999) Lexical access routes to nouns in a morphologically rich language. Journal of Memory and Language, 401, 109–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lexicon of Common Words in Contemporary Chinese
(2008) Beijing: The Commercial Press.Google Scholar
Li, H., & Chen, H-C.
(1999) Radical processing in Chinese character recognition: Evidence from illusory conjunction. Psychological Science (China), 221, 213–217.Google Scholar
Li, X. S., Rayner, K., & Cave, K.
(2009) On the segmentation of Chinese words during reading. Cognitive Psychology, 581, 525–552. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y., & Peng, D.
(1997) Meaning access of Chinese compounds and its time course. In Hsuan-Chih Chen (Eds.), Cognitive processing of Chinese and related Asian languages (pp. 219–232). Hong Kong: The Chinese University Press.Google Scholar
Mattingly, I. G., & Xu, Y.
(1993) Word superiority in Chinese. Haskins Laboratories Status Report on Speech Research, SR-l13, 145–152.Google Scholar
Myers, J.
(2006) Processing Chinese compounds: A survey of the literature. In G. Libben & G. Jarema (Eds.), The representation and processing of compound words (pp. 169–196). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
in press). Processing of Chinese compounds. In R. Sybesma, W. Behr, Y. Gu, Z. Handel, C.-T. J. Huang, & J. Myers Eds. Encyclopedia of Chinese Language and Linguistics Leiden, Netherlands Brill
Pollatsek, A., Hyönä, J., & Bertram, R.
(2000) The role of morphological constituents in reading Finnish compound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 261, 820–833.Google Scholar
R Core Team
(2016) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL [URL].
Rayner, K.
(1975) The perceptual span and peripheral cues during reading. Cognitive Psychology, 71, 65–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1998) Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 1241, 372–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H.
(1995) Modeling morphological processing. In L. B. Feldman (Eds.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 131–156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Taft, M., & Forster, K. I.
(1976) Lexical storage and retrieval of polymorphemic and polysyllabic words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 151, 607–620. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taft, M., Huang, J. & Zhu, X.
(1994) The influence of character frequency on word recognition responses in Chinese. In H.-W. Chang, J. T. Huang, C.-W. Hue & O. Tzeng (Eds.), Advances in the study of Chinese language processing. Taipei: Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University.Google Scholar
Yan, G., Tian, H., Bai, X., & Rayner, K.
(2006) The effect of word and character frequency on the eye movements of Chinese readers. British Journal of Psychology, 971, 259–268. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yeh, S.-L., & Li, J.-L.
(2002) Role of structure and component in judgments of visual similarity of Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 281, 933–947.Google Scholar
Zhou, X. Marslen-Wilson, W., Taft, M., & Shu, H.
(1999) Morphology, orthography, and phonology in reading Chinese compound words. Language and Cognitive Processes, 141, 525–565. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Bonin, Patrick, Betty Laroche & Alain Méot
2021. Psycholinguistic norms for a set of 506 French compound words. Behavior Research Methods 54:1  pp. 393 ff. DOI logo
Hyönä, Jukka, Lei Cui, Timo T. Heikkilä, Birgitta Paranko, Yun Gao & Xingzhi Su
2024. Reading compound words in Finnish and Chinese: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Memory and Language 134  pp. 104474 ff. DOI logo
Wei, Yanjun, Ying Niu, Marcus Taft & Manuel Carreiras
2023. Morphological decomposition in Chinese compound word recognition: Electrophysiological evidence. Brain and Language 241  pp. 105267 ff. DOI logo
Zhang, Zhongpei
2024. Frequency effects can modulate the neural correlates of prosodic processing in Mandarin. NeuroReport 35:6  pp. 399 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.