To what extent do speakers decompose morphologically complex words, such as segmentable, into their
morphological constituents? In this article, we argue that spelling errors in English affixes reflect morphological boundary
strength and degrees of segmentability. In support of this argument, we present a case study examining the spelling of the
suffixes -able/-ible, -ence/-ance, and -ment in an online resource (Tweets), in
forms such as <availible>, <invisable>, <eloquance>, and <bettermint>. Based on previous research on
morphological productivity and boundary strength (Hay, 2002; Hay & Baayen, 2002, 2005), we hypothesized that
morphological segmentability should affect the choice between <able> vs. <ible>, <ance> vs. <ence>, and
<ment> vs. <-mint>. An analysis of roughly 23,000 non-standard spellings is consistent with that hypothesis,
underscoring the usefulness of spelling variation as a source of evidence for morphological segmentability and for the role of
morphological representations in language production and comprehension.
Adams, V. (2001). Complex words in English. Longman.
Assink, E. M. (1985). Assessing spelling strategies for the orthography of Dutch verbs. British Journal of Psychology, 76(3), 353–363.
Baayen, R. H. (2014). Experimental and psycholinguistic approaches to studying derivation. Handbook of derivational morphology, 95–117.
Baayen, R. H., Feldman, L. B., & Schreuder, R. (2006). Morphological influences on the recognition of monosyllabic monomorphemic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(2), 290–313.
Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1995). The CELEX lexical database (release 2). Distributed by the Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.
Badecker, W., Hillis, A., & Caramazza, A. (1990). Lexical morphology and its role in the writing process: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia. Cognition, 35(3), 205–243.
Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Cortese, M. J., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., … Treiman, R. (2007). The English Lexicon Project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459.
Bar-On, A., & Kuperman, V. (2019). Spelling errors respect morphology: a corpus study of Hebrew orthography. Reading and Writing, 32.5, 1107–1128.
Bauer, L., Lieber, R., & Plag, I. (2013). The Oxford reference guide to English morphology. Oxford University Press. Retrieved from
Baus, C., Strijkers, K., & Costa, A. (2013). When does word frequency influence written production?Frontiers in Psychology, 41. Retrieved from
Bertram, R., Tønnessen, F. E., Strömqvist, S., Hyönä, J., & Niemi, P. (2015). Cascaded processing in written compound word production. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 207.
Bloomer, R. H. (1956). Word length and complexity variables in spelling difficulty. The Journal of Educational Research, 49 (7), 531–536.
Blumenthal-Dramé, A., Glauche, V., Bormann, T., Weiller, C., Musso, M., & Kortmann, B. (2017). Frequency and chunking in derived words: a parametric fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond kučera and francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41 (4), 977–990.
Cahen, L. S., Craun, M. J., & Johnson, S. K. (1971). Spelling diffculty: A survey of the research. Review of Educational Research, 41 (4), 281–301.
Caramazza, A., Miceli, G., Villa, G., & Romani, C. (1987). The role of the graphemic buffer in spelling: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia. Cognition, 26 (1), 59–85.
Carlisle, J. F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth, and eighth graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9 (3), 247–266.
Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English.
Cohen, C. (2014). Probabilistic reduction and probabilistic enhancement. Morphology, 24 (4), 291–323.
Crump, M. J. C., & Logan, G. D. (2010). Warning: This keyboard will deconstruct – the role of the keyboard in skilled typewriting. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17 (3), 394–399. Retrieved from
Cutler, A. (2011). Slips of the tongue and language production. Walter de Gruyter.
Davies, M. (2013). The Corpus of Contemporary American English (full text on CD): 440 million words, 1990–2012.
Delattre, M., Bonin, P., & Barry, C. (2006). Written spelling to dictation: Sound-to-spelling regularity affects both writing latencies and durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(6), 1330.
Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological review, 93(3), 283.
Deorowicz, S., & Ciura, M. G. (2005). Correcting spelling errors by modelling their causes. International Journal of Applied Mathematics and Computer Science, 151, 275–285.
Dressler, W. (1985). Morphonology. Ann Arbor: Karoma.
Falkauskas, K., & Kuperman, V. (2015). When experience meets language statistics: Individual variability in processing English compound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(6), 1607.
Fayol, M., Largy, P., & Lemaire, P. (1994). Cognitive overload and orthographic errors: When cognitive overload enhances subject–verb agreement errors. a study in French written language. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47(2), 437–464.
Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2014). Typing time as an index of morphological and semantic effects during English compound processing. Lingue e linguaggio, 13(2), 241–262.
Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2016a). Effects of morphology and semantic transparency on typing latencies in English compound and pseudocompound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(9), 1489.
Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2016b). Written production of English compounds: effects of morphology and semantic transparency. Morphology, 26(2), 133–155.
Gentry, J. (2015). twitter: R based twitter client [Computer software manual]. Retrieved from [URL] (R package version 1.1.9)
Hay, J. (2001). Lexical frequency in morphology: is everything relative?Linguistics, 39 (6), 1041–1070.
Hay, J. (2002). From speech perception to morphology: Affix ordering revisited. Language, 78 (3), 527–555.
Hay, J. (2003). Causes and Consequences of Word Structure. New York: Routledge. Retrieved from
Hay, J. (2007). The phonetics of ‘un’. Lexical creativity, texts and contexts, 39–57.
Hay, J., & Baayen, H. (2002). Parsing and productivity. In Yearbook of Morphology (pp. 203–235). Springer Netherlands.
Hay, J., & Baayen, H. (2005). Shifting paradigms: gradient structure in morphology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9 (7), 342–348.
Hay, J., & Plag, I. (2004). What constrains possible suffix combinations? On the interaction of grammatical and processing restrictions in derivational morphology. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 22 (3), 565–596.
Kawaletz, L., & Plag, I. (2015). Predicting the semantics of English nominalizations: a frame-based analysis of -ment suffixation. In L. Bauer, P. Stekauer, & L. Kortvelyessy (Eds.), Semantics of Complex Words (pp. 289–319). Dordrecht: Springer.
Kemps, R. J. J. K., Ernestus, M., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2005). Prosodic cues for morphological complexity: The case of Dutch plural nouns. Memory & Cognition, 33 (3), 430–446.
Kiparsky, P. (1982). Lexical morphology and phonology. In I.-S. Yang (Ed.), Linguistics in the Morning Calm: Selected Papers from SICOL (pp. 3–91). Seoul: Hanshin.
Kuperman, V., & Bertram, R. (2013). Moving spaces: Spelling alternation in English noun-noun compounds. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28 (7), 939–966. Retrieved from
Kuperman, V., Pluymaekers, M., Ernestus, M., & Baayen, H. (2007). Morphological predictability and acoustic duration of interfixes in Dutch compounds. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 121(4), 2261–2271.
Lambert, E., Kandel, S., Fayol, M., & Espéret, E. (2008). The effect of the number of syllables on handwriting production. Reading and Writing, 21(9), 859–883.
Largy, P. (1996). The homophone effect in written French: The case of verb-noun inflection errors. Language and cognitive processes, 11(3), 217–256.
Lee-Kim, S.-I., Davidson, L., & Hwang, S. (2013). Morphological effects on the darkness of English intervocalic /l/. Laboratory Phonology, 4(2), 475–511.
Libben, G., Jarema, G., & Luke, J. (May, 2018). Same words, different languages: Examining English-French written word recognition and production. Annual Meeting of the Canadian Linguistics Association, Regina, Canada. [URL]
Mahony, D. L. (1994). Using sensitivity to word structure to explain variance in high school and college level reading ability. Reading and Writing, 6(1), 19–44.
Marchand, H. (1969). The categories and types of present-day English word-formation (2nd ed.). München: Verlag C. H. Beck.
Nottbusch, G., Grimm, A., Weingarten, R., & Will, U. (2005). Syllabic sructures in typing: Evidence from deaf writers. Reading and Writing, 18(6), 497–526.
Plag, I. (2003). Word-formation in English. Cambridge University Press. Retrieved from
Plag, I. (2014). Phonological and phonetic variability in complex words: An uncharted territory. Italian Journal of Linguistics/Rivista di Linguistica, 26(2), 209–228.
Plag, I., & Baayen, R. H. (2009). Suffix ordering and morphological processing. Language, 851, 106–149.
Plag, I., & Ben Hedia, S. (2018). The phonetics of newly derived words: Testing the effect of morphological segmentability on affix duration. In S. Arndt-Lappe, A. Braun, C. Moulin, & E. Winter-Froemel (Eds.), Expanding the Lexicon: Linguistic Innovation, Morphological Productivity, and the Role of Discourse-related Factors (pp. 93–116). Berlin, New York: de Gruyter Mouton.
R Development Core Team. (2008). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software manual]. Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from [URL] (ISBN 3-900051-07-0)
Rahmanian, S., & Kuperman, V. (2019). Spelling errors impede recognition of correctly spelled word forms. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23 (1), 24–36.
Rapp, B., & Fischer-Baum, S. (2014). Representation of orthographic knowledge. The Oxford handbook of language production, 3381.
Roux, S., McKeeff, T. J., Grosjacques, G., Afonso, O., & Kandel, S. (2013). The interaction between central and peripheral processes in handwriting production. Cognition, 127 (2), 235–241.
Sahel, S., Nottbusch, G., Grimm, A., & Weingarten, R. (2008). Written production of German compounds: Effects of lexical frequency and semantic transparency. Written Language & Literacy, 11 (2), 211–227.
Sandra, D. (2010). Homophone dominance at the whole-word and sub-word levels: Spelling errors suggest full-form storage of regularly inflected verb forms. Language and speech, 53 (3), 405–444.
Sandra, D., & Fayol, M. (2003). Spelling errors with a view on the mental lexicon: Frequency and proximity effects in misspelling homophonous regular verb forms in Dutch and French. Trends in Linguistics Studies and Monographs, 1511, 485–514.
Sandra, D., Frisson, S., & Daems, F. (1999). Why simple verb forms can be so difficult to spell: The influence of homophone frequency and distance in Dutch. Brain and language, 68 (1–2), 277–283.
Scaltritti, M., Arfé, B., Torrance, M., & Peressotti, F. (2016). Typing pictures: Linguistic processing cascades into finger movements. Cognition, 1561, 16–29. Retrieved from
Seyfarth, S., Garellek, M., Gillingham, G., Ackerman, F., & Malouf, R. (2017). Acoustic differences in morphologically-distinct homophones. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 1–18.
Siegel, D. (1979). Topics in English morphology. Garland.
Singson, M., Mahony, D., & Mann, V. (2000). The relation between reading ability and morphological skills: Evidence from derivational suffixes. Reading and writing, 12 (3), 219–252.
Smith, R., Baker, R., & Hawkins, S. (2012). Phonetic detail that distinguishes prefixed from pseudo-prefixed words. Journal of Phonetics, 40 (5), 689–705. Retrieved from {[URL]}.
Solso, R. L., & Juel, C. L. (1980). Positional frequency and versatility of bigrams for two-through nine-letter English words. Behavior Research Methods, 12 (3), 297–343.
Spencer, K. (2007). Predicting children’s word-spelling difficulty for common English words from measures of orthographic transparency, phonemic and graphemic length and word frequency. British Journal of Psychology, 98(2), 305–338.
Sproat, R., & Fujimura, O. (1993). Allophonic variation in English /l/ and its implications for phonetic implementation. Journal of Phonetics, 211, 291–311.
Vannest, J., Newport, E. L., Newman, A. J., & Bavelier, D. (2011). Interplay between morphology and frequency in lexical access: The case of the base frequency effect. Brain Research, 13731, 144–159.
Weingarten, R., Nottbusch, G., & Will, U. (2004). Morphemes, syllables and graphemes in written word production. In T. Pechmann & C. Habel (Eds.), Multidisciplinary approaches to language production (pp. 529–572). Mouton de Gruyter.
Wikipedia. (2017). Wikipedia:lists of common misspellings — Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia. Retrieved from {[URL]} ([Online; accessed 04 September 2017])
Zirkel, L. (2010). Prefix combinations in English: Structural and processing factors. Morphology, 20(1), 239–266.
Cited by (7)
Cited by seven other publications
Muschalik, Julia & Gero Kunter
2024. Do letters matter? The influence of spelling on acoustic duration. Phonetica 81:2 ► pp. 221 ff.
2024. Typing /s/—morphology between the keys?. Reading and Writing
Sandra, Dominiek, Dorit Ravid & Ingo Plag
2024. The orthographic representation of a word’s morphological structure: beneficial and detrimental effect for spellers. Morphology 34:2 ► pp. 103 ff.
Sandra, Dominiek
2022. Too Little Morphology Can Kill You: The Interplay Between Low-Frequency Morpho-Orthographic Rules and High-Frequency Verb Homophones in Spelling Errors. In Developing Language and Literacy [Literacy Studies, 23], ► pp. 191 ff.
2021. 2021 7th International Conference on Education and Training Technologies, ► pp. 96 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 december 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.