Article published In:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 14:3 (2019) ► pp.381398
References

References

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S.
(2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using {lme4}. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Breitenstein, C., Jansen, A., Deppe, M., Foerster, A.-F., Sommer, J., Wolbers, T., & Knecht, S.
(2005) Hippocampus activity differentiates good from poor learners of a novel lexicon. NeuroImage, 25(3), 958–968. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davis, M. H., Di Betta, A. M., Macdonald, M. J., & Gaskell, M. G.
(2009) Learning and consolidation of novel spoken words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(4), 803–820. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davis, M. H., & Gaskell, M. G.
(2009) A complementary systems account of word learning: neural and behavioural evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1536), 3773–3800. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eichenbaum, H.
(2012) The cognitive neuroscience of memory: an introduction (2nd ed). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, V.
(2009) How Words Mean: Lexical Concepts, Cognitive Models, and Meaning Construction. Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logo
Gaskell, M. G., & Ellis, A. W.
(2009) Word learning and lexical development across the lifespan. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1536), 3607–3615. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M., Mervis, C. B., & Hirsh-Pasek, K.
(1994) Early object labels: The case for a developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language, 211, 125–155. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamrick, P.
(2015) Declarative and procedural memory abilities as individual differences in incidental language learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 441, 9–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamrick, P., Lum, J. A. G., & Ullman, M. T.
(2018) Child first language and adult second language are both tied to general-purpose learning systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(7), 1487–1492. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamrick, P., & Rebuschat, P.
(2012) How implicit is statistical learning? In P. Rebuschat & J. N. Williams (Eds.), Statistical learning and language acquisition. (pp. 365–382). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hamrick, P., & Sachs, R.
(2018) Establishing evidence of learning in experiments with artificial linguistic systems. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(1), 153–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T.
(2002) The Faculty of Language: What Is It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N., & Forster, K. I.
(2001) Cross-Language Priming Asymmetries in Lexical Decision and Episodic Recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(1), 32–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kensinger, E. A., Ullman, M. T., & Corkin, S.
(2001) Bilateral medial temporal lobe damage does not affect lexical or grammatical processing: evidence from amnesic patient H.M. Hippocampus, 11(4), 347–360. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Markman, E. M.
(1990) Constraints children place on word meanings. Cognitive Science, 141, 57–77. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Markson, L., & Bloom, P.
(1997) Evidence against a dedicated system for word learning in children. Nature, 385(6619), 813–815. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., Brill-Schuetz, K. A., Carpenter, H., & Wong, P. C. M.
(2014) Declarative and procedural memory as individual differences in second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 56–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pufahl, A., & Samuel, A. G.
(2014) How lexical is the lexicon? Evidence for integrated auditory memory representations. Cognitive Psychology, 701, 1–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Qiao, X. & Forster, K. I.
(2013) Novel word lexicalization and the prime lexicality effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(4), 1064–1074.Google Scholar
Qiao, X., & Forster, K. I.
(2017) Is the L2 lexicon different from the L1 lexicon? Evidence from novel word lexicalization. Cognition, 1581, 147–152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Qiao, X., Forster, K. I., & Witzel, N.
(2009) Is banara really a word? Cognition, 1131, 254–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. B.
(2001) How domain-general processes may create domain-specific biases. In M. Bowerman & S. Levinson (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development. (pp. 101–131). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Takashima, A., Bakker, I., van Hell, J. G., Janzen, G., & McQueen, J. M.
(2017) Interaction between episodic and semantic memory networks in the acquisition and consolidation of novel spoken words. Brain and Language, 1671, 44–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trahan, D. E., & Larrabee, G. J.
(1988) Continuous Visual Memory Test. Odessa, FL: Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T.
(2004) Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92(1–2), 231–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) The biocognition of the mental lexicon. Retrieved from [URL]
(2015) The declarative/procedural model: A neurobiologically motivated theory of first and second language. In Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (2nd ed., pp. 135–158). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
(2016) The Declarative/Procedural Model. In Neurobiology of Language (pp. 953–968). Retrieved from [URL]. DOI logo
Ullman, M. T., Lum, J. A. G., & Conti-Ramsden, G. M.
(2014) Domain specificity in language development. In P. Brooks & V. Kempe (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Languages Development (pp. 163–166). Los Angeles: Sage.Google Scholar
Witzel, N. O., & Forster, K. I.
(2012) How L2 words are stored: the episodic L2 hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(6), 1608–1621. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yee, E., Jones, M., & McRae, K.
(2018) Semantic memory. In J. Wixted & S. Thompson-Schill (Eds.), The Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience (4th ed., Vol. 31, pp. 319–356). New York: Wiley. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Murphy, Josiah, Ryan T. Miller & Phillip Hamrick
Zhang, Yin, Maryna Ridchenko, Aimi Hayashi & Phillip Hamrick
2021. Episodic memory contributions to second language lexical development persist at higher proficiencies. Applied Cognitive Psychology 35:5  pp. 1356 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 10 april 2022. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.