Article published In:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 17:2 (2022) ► pp.213238
References (61)
References
Abu-Rabia, S., & Kehat, S. (2004). The critical period for second language pronunciation: is there such a thing? Educational Psychology, 24 (1), 77–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adler, R. B., & Proctor II, R. F. (2017). Looking out. Looking in. Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Alemán Bañón, J., Fiorentino, R., & Gabriele, A. (2018). Using event-related potentials to track morphosyntactic development in second language learners: The processing of number and gender agreement in Spanish. PloS one, 13 (7), e0200791. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, A., Bulkes, N., & Tanner, D. (2018). Quantificational cues modulate the processing of English subject-verb agreement by native Chinese speakers: an ERP study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40 (4), 731–754. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J. (2008). Analyzing ‘visual world’ eyetracking data using multilevel logistic regression. Journal of Memory and Language, 59 (4), 457–474. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4 . arXiv Preprint arXiv:1406.5823.Google Scholar
Beatty-Martı́nez, A. L., Navarro-Torres, C. A., Dussias, P. E., Bajo, M. T., Guzzardo Tamargo, R. E., & Kroll, J. F. (2020). Interactional context mediates the consequences of bilingualism for language and cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46 (6), 1022–1047. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berghoff, R., McLoughlin, J., & Bylund, E. (2021). L1 activation during L2 processing is modulated by both age of acquisition and proficiency. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 58 1, 100979. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Black, M., Joanisse, M. F., & Rafat, Y. (2020). Language Dominance Modulates the Perception of Spanish Approximants in Late Bilinguals. Languages, 5 (1), 7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bonfieni, M., Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & Sorace, A. (2019). Language experience modulates bilingual language control: The effect of proficiency, age of acquisition, and exposure on language switching. Acta psychologica, 193 1, 160–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cohen, N. J. & Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowin how and knowing that. Science, 210 (4466), 207–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. L. (1985). Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form, vol. 91. John Benjamins Publishing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cholin, J., Levelt, W. J., & Schiller, N. O. (2006). Effects of syllable frequency in speech production. Cognition, 99 1, 205–235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Clopper, C. G. (2002). Frequency of stress patterns in English: A computational analysis. IULC Working Papers Online, 2 (2), 1–9.Google Scholar
Cooper, N., Cutler, A., & Wales, R. (2002). Constraints of lexical stress on lexical access in English: Evidence from native and non-native listeners. Language and Speech, 45 (3), 207–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1986). The syllable’s differing role in the segmentation of French and English. Journal of memory and language, 25 (4), 385–400. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Del Maschio, N., Sulpizio, S., Toti, M., Caprioglio, C., Del Mauro, G., Fedeli, D., & Abutalebi, J. (2020). Second language use rather than second language knowledge relates to changes in white matter microstructure. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, 4 1, 165–175. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeLuca, V., Segaert, K., Mazaheri, A., & Krott, A. (2020). Understanding bilingual brain function and structure changes? U Bet! A Unified Bilingual Experience Trajectory model. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56 1, 100930. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Faretta-Stutenberg, M., & Morgan-Short, K. (2018). The interplay of individual differences and context of learning in behavioral and neurocognitive second language development. Second Language Research, 34 (1), 67–101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fischer, B. (1992). Saccadic reaction time: Implications for reading, dyslexia, and visual cognition. In Rayner, K. (Ed.), Eye Movements and Visual Cognition. Springer Series in Neuropsychology (pp. 192–202). Springer, New York, NY. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Bohn, O. S. (2021). The revised speech learning model (SLM-r). Second Language Speech Learning: Theoretical and Empirical Progress, 3–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., & MacKay, I. R. (2004). Perceiving vowels in a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26 (1), 1–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gonnerman, L. M., Seidenberg, M. S., & Andersen, E. S. (2007). Graded semantic and phonological similarity effects in priming: evidence for a distributed connectionist approach to morphology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136 (2), 323. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gosselke Berthelsen, S., Horne, M., Brännström, K. J., Shtyrov, Y., & Roll, M. (2018). Neural processing of morphosyntactic tonal cues in second-language learners. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 45 1, 60–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guion, S., Flege, J., Liu, S., & Yeni-Komshian, G. (2000). Age of learning effects on the duration of sentences produced in a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21 (2), 205–228. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H. (2016). Learning (not) to predict: Grammatical gender processing in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 32 (2), 277–307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hualde, J. I. (2005). The sounds of Spanish with audio CD. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Isabelli-García, C., & Lacorte, M. (2016). Language learners’ characteristics, target language use, and linguistic development in a domestic immersion context. Foreign Language Annals, 49 (3), 544–556. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ito, A., Dunn, M. S., & Pickering, M. (2017). Effects of language production on prediction: Word vs. picture visual world study. (Poster) Architectures and mechanisms of language processing.Google Scholar
Jun, S. A., & Oh, M. (2000). Acquisition of second language intonation. In Sixth International Conference on Spoken Language Processing. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kelly, M. H., & Bock, J. K. (1988). Stress in time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14 (3), 389. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kimppa, L., Shtyrov, Y., Hut, S. C., Hedlund, L., Leminen, M., & Leminen, A. (2019). Acquisition of L2 morphology by adult language learners. Cortex, 116 1, 74–90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Konishi, T., Yun, J., & Kondo, M. (2018). Acoustic correlates of L2 English stress ― Comparison of Japanese English and Korean English. Phonetics Speech Sci., 10 (1), 9–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kostić, A., Markovic, T., & Baucal, A. (2003). Inflectional morphology and word meaning: Orthogonal or co-implicative cognitive domains? Trends in Linguistics Studies and Monographs, 151 1, 1–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lloyd-Smith, A., Einfeldt, M., & Kupisch, T. (2020). Italian-German bilinguals: The effects of heritage language use on accent in early-acquired languages. International Journal of Bilingualism, 24 (2), 289–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lozano-Argüelles, C., Sagarra, N., & Casillas, J. V. (2020). Slowly but surely: Interpreting facilitates L2 morphological anticipation based on suprasegmental and segmental information. Special issue “Interpreting: A Window to Bilingual Processing” (Eds. Yanping Dong and Ping Li). Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 231, 752–762. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lüdecke, D., Ben-Shachar, M. S., Patil, I., Waggoner, P., & Makowski, D. (2021). performance: An R package for assessment, comparison and testing of statistical models. Journal of Open Source Software, 6 (60). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maddah, Z. G., Reiterer, S. M. (2018). Language Transfer vs. Language Talent? Individual Differences and Aptitude in L2 Phonology of Persian-Speaking Learners of English. In: Reiterer, S. (Eds.), Exploring Language Aptitude: Views from Psychology, the Language Sciences, and Cognitive Neuroscience. English Language Education, vol. 161 (pp. 363–388). Springer, Cham. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mirman, D. (2016). Growth curve analysis and visualization using R. CRC press.Google Scholar
Morales-Font, A. (2014). El acento [The accent]. In R. A. Núñez., S. Colina., & T. G. Bradley (Eds.). Fonología generativa contemporánea de la lengua española [Contemporary generative phonology of the Spanish language] (2nd Edition) (pp. 235–265). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (2014). Contrasting effects of starting age and input on the oral performance of foreign language learners. Applied Linguistics, 35 (4), 463–482. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ortega-Llebaria, M. (2006). Phonetic cues to stress and accent in Spanish. In Selected proceedings of the 2nd conference on laboratory approaches to spanish phonetics and phonology (pp. 104–118). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Perdomo, M., & Kaan, E. (2019). Prosodic cues in second-language speech processing: A visual world eye-tracking study. Second Language Research, 37 (2), 349–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from [URL]
Ranta, L., & Meckelborg, A. (2013). How much exposure to English do international graduate students really get? Measuring language use in a naturalistic setting. Canadian Modern Language Review, 69 (1), 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Real Academia Española. Banco de datos (CORPES XXI) [online]. Corpus del Español del Siglo XXI (CORPES). <[URL]> [February 11, 2022]
Roll, M., Horne, M., & Lindgren, M. (2011). Activating without inhibiting: Left-edge boundary tones and syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23 (5), 1170–1179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roll, M., Söderström, P., Frid, J., Mannfolk, P. & Horne, M. (2017). Forehearing words: Pre-activation of word endings at word onset. Neuroscience Letters, 658 1, 57–61. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roll, M., Söderström, P., Mannfolk, P., Shtyrov, Y., Johansson, M., Westen, D. V., & Horne, M. (2015). Word tones cueing morphosyntactic structure: Neuroanatomical substrates and activation time-course assessed by EEG and fMRI. Brain and Language, 150 1, 14–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Romberg, A. R., & Saffran, J. R. (2010). Statistical learning and language acquisition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1 (6), 906–914. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rossi, S., Gugler, M. F., Friederici, A. D., & Hahne, A. (2006). The impact of proficiency on syntactic second-language processing of German and Italian: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18 (12), 2030–2048. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sagarra, N. (2014). Absence of morphological transfer in beginners. In Z. Han and R. Rast (Eds), First exposure to a second language. Learners’ initial input processing (pp. 139–170). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2021). When more is better: Higher L1-L2 similarity, L2 proficiency, and working memory facilitate L2 morphosyntactic processing. In M. Leeser, G. Keating, & W. Wong (Eds.), Research on Second Language Processing and Processing Instruction (pp. 125–150) (Studies in Bilingualism; Vol. 62). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sagarra, N. & Casillas, J. V. (2018). The use of stress in L1 and L2 lexical access. Special issue on Second Language Processing (Ed. Jiang). Journal of Second Language Studies, 1(1), 31–60. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schremm, A., Söderström, P., Horne, M., & Roll, M. (2016). Implicit acquisition of tone-suffix connections in L2 learners of Swedish. The Mental Lexicon, 11 (1), 55–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. The Bell system technical journal, 27 (3), 379–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soto-Faraco, S., Sebastián-Gallés, N., & Cutler, A. (2001). Segmental and suprasegmental mismatch in lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language, 45 1, 412–432. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Leussen, J.-W., & Escudero, P. (2015). Learning to perceive and recognize a second language: the L2LP model revised. Frontiers in Psychology. 6 1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, E. J., Titone, D., Genesee, F., & Steinhauer, K. (2015). Phonological processing in late second language learners: The effects of proficiency and task. Bilingualism, 20 (1), 162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yao, P., & Chen, B. (2017). Cross-linguistic differences affect late Chinese-English learners’ on-line processing of English tense and aspect. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21 (3), 268–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar